ÌÇÐÄvlog

[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.Ìý

Effects of ketamine administration on mismatch negativity (MMN) (difference wave) in the pitch (A) and duration (B) deviance condition (N = 20). The gray lines represent the MMN wave during the baseline recording before ketamine administration; the black line, the MMN wave during infusion of ketamine; and the dark area, the difference between the 2 curves (ie, the reduction of MMN during ketamine administration).

Figure 2.Ìý

Effects of ketamine infusion on performance on the "A-X" version of the Continuous Performance Test at the short interstimulus interval (n = 19). Error bar values are mean + SEM. In the 4 panels, the hit and alarm rates, respectively, to the 4 possible combinations of cue and target stimuli, are presented.

Figure 3.Ìý

Effects of ketamine infusion on performance on the "A-X" version of the Continuous Performance Test at the long interstimulus interval (n = 19). Error bar values are mean + SEM.

Table 1.Ìý
Peak Amplitudes and Latencies of Mismatch Negativity (MMN) to Pitch and Duration Deviants (N = 20)
Table 2.Ìý
Peak Amplitudes and Latencies of Sensory Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) N1 and P2 (N = 20)
1.
Bilder ÌýRMÌýThe neuropsychology of schizophrenia: what, when, where, how?ÌýFleischhacker ÌýWWHinterhuber ÌýHMeise ÌýUeds.ÌýSchizophrene Störungen State of the Art II, Ursachen-Behandlung-Verlauf Innsbruck, Switzerland Verlag Integrative Psychiatrie1998;155-Ìý171
2.
Pantelis ÌýCNelson ÌýHEBarnes ÌýTREÌýSchizophrenia: A Neuropsychological Perspective.Ìý New York, NY John Wiley & Sons Inc1998;
3.
Cornblatt ÌýBAKeilp ÌýJGÌýImpaired attention, genetics, and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.ÌýÌýSchizophr Bull. 1994;2031-Ìý46
4.
Pritchard ÌýWSÌýCognitive event-related potential correlates of schizophrenia.ÌýÌýPsychol Bull. 1986;10043-Ìý66
5.
Javitt ÌýDCDoneshka ÌýPGrochowski ÌýSRitter ÌýWÌýImpaired mismatch negativity generation reflects widespread dysfunction of working memory in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52550-Ìý558
6.
Braff ÌýDLGeyer ÌýMAÌýSensorimotor gating and schizophrenia: human and animal model studies.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47181-Ìý188
7.
Braff ÌýDLGrillon ÌýCGeyer ÌýMAÌýGating and habituation of the startle reflex in schizophrenic patients.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49206-Ìý215
8.
Green ÌýMFÌýWhat are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia?ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1996;153321-Ìý330
9.
Bilder ÌýRMÌýNeurocognitive impairment in schizophrenia and how it affects treatment options.ÌýÌýCan J Psychiatry. 1997;42255-Ìý264
10.
Goldman ÌýRSAxelrod ÌýBNTandon ÌýRRibeiro ÌýSCCraig ÌýKBerent ÌýSÌýNeuropsychological prediction of treatment efficacy and one-year outcome in schizophrenia.ÌýÌý±Ê²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è²¹³Ù³ó´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1993;26122-Ìý126
11.
Lu ÌýZLWilliamson ÌýSJKaufman ÌýLÌýBehavioral lifetime of human auditory sensory memory predicted by physiological measures.ÌýÌý³§³¦¾±±ð²Ô³¦±ð. 1992;2581668-Ìý1670
12.
Strous ÌýRDCowan ÌýNRitter ÌýWJavitt ÌýDCÌýAuditory sensory ("echoic") memory dysfunction in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1995;1521517-Ìý1519
13.
Javitt ÌýDCStrous ÌýRDGrochowski ÌýSRitter ÌýWCowan ÌýNÌýImpaired precision, but normal retention, of auditory sensory ("echoic") memory information in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýJ Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106315-Ìý324
14.
Wexler ÌýBEStevens ÌýAABowers ÌýAASernyak ÌýMJGoldman-Rakic ÌýPSÌýWord and tone working memory deficits in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;551093-Ìý1096
15.
Catts ÌýSVÌýBrain potential evidence for an auditory sensory memory deficit in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1995;152213-Ìý219
16.
Javitt ÌýDCDoneshka ÌýPZylberman ÌýIRitter ÌýWVaughan ÌýHG ÌýJrÌýImpairment of early cortical processing in schizophrenia: an event-related potential confirmation study.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1993;33513-Ìý519
17.
Oades ÌýRDZerbin ÌýDEggers ÌýCÌýStimulus-Vergleichsprozesse bei psychotischen Jugendlichen mit paranoiden und nicht paranoiden Symptomen: "Mismatch Negativity" deutet auf differenzierte Beeinträchtigungen hin.ÌýBaumann ÌýPed.ÌýBiologische Psychiatrie der Gegenwart New York, NY Springer-Verlag NY Inc1993;69-Ìý74
18.
Shelley ÌýAMWard ÌýPBCatts ÌýSVMichie ÌýPTAndrews ÌýSMcConaghy ÌýNÌýMismatch negativity: an index of a preattentive processing deficit in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1991;301059-Ìý1062
19.
Umbricht ÌýDJavitt ÌýDCNovak ÌýGPBates ÌýJAPollack ÌýSLieberman ÌýJKane ÌýJÌýEffects of clozapine on auditory event-related potentials in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1998;44716-Ìý725
20.
Umbricht ÌýDJavitt ÌýDCNovak ÌýGPBates ÌýJAPollak ÌýSLieberman ÌýJKane ÌýJÌýEffects of risperidone on auditory event-related potentials in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýInt J Neuropsychopharmacol. 1999;2229-Ìý304
21.
Javitt ÌýDCShelley ÌýAMSilipo ÌýGLieberman ÌýJAÌýDeficits in auditory and visual context-dependent process ing in schizophrenia: defining the pattern.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;571131-Ìý1137
22.
Servan-Schreiber ÌýDCohen ÌýJDSteingard ÌýSÌýSchizophrenic deficits in the processing of context: a test of a theoretical model.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;531105-Ìý1112
23.
Cohen ÌýJDBarch ÌýDMCarter ÌýCServan-Schreiber ÌýDÌýContext-processing deficits in schizophrenia: converging evidence from three theoretically motivated cognitive tasks.ÌýÌýJ Abnorm Psychol. 1999;108120-Ìý133
24.
Olney ÌýJWFarber ÌýNBÌýGlutamate receptor dysfunction and schizophrenia.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52998-Ìý1007
25.
Javitt ÌýDCZukin ÌýSRÌýRecent advances in the phencyclidine model of schizophrenia.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1991;1481301-Ìý1308
26.
Krystal ÌýJHKarper ÌýLPSeibyl ÌýJPFreeman ÌýGKDelaney ÌýRBremner ÌýJDHeninger ÌýGRBowers ÌýMB ÌýJrCharney ÌýDSÌýSubanesthetic effects of the noncompetitive NMDA antagonist, ketamine, in humans: psychotomimetic, perceptual, cognitive, and neuroendocrine responses.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51199-Ìý214
27.
Adler ÌýCMMalhotra ÌýAKElman ÌýIGoldberg ÌýTEgan ÌýMPickar ÌýDBreier ÌýAÌýComparison of ketamine-induced thought disorder in healthy volunteers and thought disorder in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1999;1561646-Ìý1649
28.
Adler ÌýCMGoldberg ÌýTEMalhotra ÌýAKPickar ÌýDBreier ÌýAÌýEffects of ketamine on thought disorder, working memory, and semantic memory in healthy volunteers.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1998;43811-Ìý816
29.
Bakshi ÌýVPSwerdlow ÌýNRGeyer ÌýMAÌýClozapine antagonizes phencyclidine-induced deficits in sensorimotor gating of the startle response.ÌýÌýJ Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1994;271787-Ìý794
30.
Bakshi ÌýVPGeyer ÌýMAÌýAntagonism of phencyclidine-induced deficits in prepulse inhibition by the putative atypical antipsychotic olanzapine.ÌýÌý±Ê²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²¹°ù³¾²¹³¦´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1995;122198-Ìý201
31.
Swerdlow ÌýNRBakshi ÌýVGeyer ÌýMAÌýSeroquel restores sensorimotor gating in phencyclidine-treated rats.ÌýÌýJ Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1996;2791290-Ìý1299
32.
Breier ÌýAMalhotra ÌýAKPinals ÌýDAWeisenfeld ÌýNIPickar ÌýDÌýAssociation of ketamine-induced psychosis with focal activation of the prefrontal cortex in healthy volunteers.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1997;154805-Ìý811
33.
Vollenweider ÌýFXLeenders ÌýKLOye ÌýIHell ÌýDAngst ÌýJÌýDifferential psychopathology and patterns of cerebral glucose utilisation produced by (S)- and (R)-ketamine in healthy volunteers using positron emission tomography (PET).ÌýÌýEur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1997;725-Ìý38
34.
Vollenweider ÌýFXLeenders ÌýKLScharfetter ÌýCAntonini ÌýAMaguire ÌýPMissimer ÌýJAngst ÌýJÌýMetabolic hyperfrontality and psychopathology in the ketamine model of psychosis using positron emission tomography (PET) and [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG).ÌýÌýEur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1997;79-Ìý24
35.
Ohno ÌýMYamamoto ÌýTWatanabe ÌýSÌýIntrahippocampal administration of a glycine site antagonist impairs working memory performance of rats.ÌýÌýEur J Pharmacol. 1994;253183-Ìý187
36.
Aura ÌýJRiekkinen ÌýP ÌýJrÌýBlockade of NMDA receptors located at the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory in rats.ÌýÌý±·±ð³Ü°ù´Ç°ù±ð±è´Ç°ù³Ù. 1999;10243-Ìý248
37.
Li ÌýHBMatsumoto ÌýKYamamoto ÌýMWatanabe ÌýHÌýNMDA but not AMPA receptor antagonists impair the delay-interposed radial maze performance of rats.ÌýÌýPharmacol Biochem Behav. 1997;58249-Ìý253
38.
Ohno ÌýMYamamoto ÌýTWatanabe ÌýSÌýEffects of intrahippocampal injections of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonists and scopolamine on working and reference memory assessed in rats by a three-panel runway task.ÌýÌýJ Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1992;263943-Ìý950
39.
Ohno ÌýMWatanabe ÌýSÌýEnhanced N-methyl-D-aspartate function reverses working memory failure induced by blockade of group I metabotropic glutamate receptors in the rat hippocampus.ÌýÌýNeurosci Lett. 1998;24037-Ìý40
40.
Javitt ÌýDCSteinschneider ÌýMSchroeder ÌýCEVaughan ÌýHG ÌýJrArezzo ÌýJCÌýDetection of stimulus deviance within primate primary auditory cortex: intracortical mechanisms of mismatch negativity (MMN) generation.ÌýÌýBrain Res. 1994;667192-Ìý200
41.
Javitt ÌýDCSteinschneider ÌýMSchroeder ÌýCEArezzo ÌýJCÌýRole of cortical N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in auditory sensory memory and mismatch negativity generation: implications for schizophrenia.ÌýÌýProc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;9311962-Ìý11967
42.
Bakker ÌýCBAmini ÌýFBÌýObservations on the psychomimetic effects of Sernyl.ÌýÌýComp Psychiatry. 1961;2269-Ìý280
43.
Ban ÌýTALohrenz ÌýJJLehmann ÌýHEÌýObservations on the action of Sernyl: a new psychotropic drug.ÌýÌýCan Psychiat Assoc J. 1961;6150-Ìý157
44.
Luby ÌýEDCohen ÌýBDRosenbaum ÌýGGottlieb ÌýJSKelley ÌýRÌýStudy of a new schizophrenomimetic drug: Sernyl.ÌýÌýArch Neurol Psychiatry. 1959;81363-Ìý369
45.
Malhotra ÌýAKPinals ÌýDAWeingartner ÌýHSirocco ÌýKMissar ÌýCDPickar ÌýDBreier ÌýAÌýNMDA receptor function and human cognition: the effects of ketamine in healthy volunteers.ÌýÌý±·±ð³Ü°ù´Ç±è²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²¹°ù³¾²¹³¦´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1996;14301-Ìý307
46.
Wieber ÌýJGugler ÌýRHengstmann ÌýJHDengler ÌýHJÌýPharmacokinetics of ketamine in man.ÌýÌý´¡²Ô²¹±ð²õ³Ù³ó±ð²õ¾±²õ³Ù. 1975;24260-Ìý263
47.
Domino ÌýEFChodoff ÌýPCorssen ÌýSÌýPharmacological effects of Cl-581, a new dissociative anesthetic in man.ÌýÌýClin Pharmacol Ther. 1965;6279-Ìý291
48.
Reich ÌýDLSilvay ÌýGÌýKetamine: an update on the first twenty-five years of clinical experience.ÌýÌýCan J Anaesth. 1989;36186-Ìý197
49.
Wittchen ÌýHSemler ÌýGÌýComposite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI): Beltz-Test, GmbH.Ìý Weinheim, Germany Psychologie Verlags Union1996;
50.
Tewes ÌýUÌýHAWIE-R: Hamburg-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Erwachsene, Revision 1991.Ìý2nd ed. Bern, Germany Hans Huber AG1994;
51.
Chapman ÌýLJChapman ÌýJPÌýThe measurement of handedness.ÌýÌýBrain Cogn. 1987;6175-Ìý183
52.
Feng ÌýNVollenweider ÌýFXMinder ÌýEIRentsch ÌýKGrampp ÌýTVonderschmitt ÌýDJÌýDevelopment of a gas chromatography-mass spectrometry method for determination of ketamine in plasma and its application to human samples.ÌýÌýTher Drug Monit. 1995;1795-Ìý100
53.
Woerner ÌýMGManuzza ÌýSKane ÌýJMÌýAnchoring the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale: an aid to improve reliability.ÌýÌýPsychopharmacol Bull. 1988;24112-Ìý117
54.
Teng ÌýELChui ÌýHCÌýThe modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) Examination.ÌýÌýJ Clin Psychiatry. 1987;48314-Ìý318
55.
Hirayasu ÌýYPotts ÌýGFO'Donnell ÌýBFKwon ÌýJSArakaki ÌýHAkdag ÌýSJLevitt ÌýJJShenton ÌýMEMcCarley ÌýRWÌýAuditory mismatch negativity in schizophrenia: topographic evaluation with a high-density recording montage.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1998;1551281-Ìý1284Google Scholar
56.
Barch ÌýDMCarter ÌýCSMacdonald ÌýASabb ÌýFWNoll ÌýDCCohen ÌýJDÌýPrefrontal cortex and context processing in medication-naive first-episode patients with schizophrenia.ÌýÌýSchizophr Res. 1999;36217-Ìý218
57.
Javitt ÌýDCSchroeder ÌýCESteinschneider ÌýMArezzo ÌýJCVaughan ÌýHG ÌýJrÌýDemonstration of mismatch negativity in the monkey.ÌýÌýElectroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1992;8387-Ìý90
58.
Sherman ÌýADHegwood ÌýTSBaruah ÌýSWaziri ÌýRÌýDeficient NMDA-mediated glutamate release from synaptosomes of schizophrenics.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1991;301191-Ìý1198
59.
Akbarian ÌýSSucher ÌýNJBradley ÌýDTafazzoli ÌýATrinh ÌýDHetrick ÌýWPPotkin ÌýSGSandman ÌýCABunney ÌýWE ÌýJrJones ÌýEGÌýSelective alterations in gene expression for NMDA receptor subunits in prefrontal cortex of schizophrenics.ÌýÌýJ Neurosci. 1996;1619-Ìý30
60.
Sokolov ÌýBPÌýExpression of NMDAR1, GluR1, GluR7, and KA1 glutamate receptor mRNAs is decreased in frontal cortex of "neuroleptic-free" schizophrenics: evidence on reversible up-regulation by typical neuroleptics.ÌýÌýJ Neurochem. 1998;712454-Ìý2464
61.
Mohn ÌýARGainetdinov ÌýRRCaron ÌýMGKoller ÌýBHÌýMice with reduced NMDA receptor expression display behaviors related to schizophrenia.ÌýÌý°ä±ð±ô±ô. 1999;98427-Ìý436
62.
Humphries ÌýCMortimer ÌýAHirsch ÌýSde Belleroche ÌýJÌýNMDA receptor mRNA correlation with antemortem cognitive impairment in schizophrenia.ÌýÌý±·±ð³Ü°ù´Ç°ù±ð±è´Ç°ù³Ù. 1996;72051-Ìý2055
63.
Lim ÌýKOHedehus ÌýMMoseley ÌýMde Crespigny ÌýASullivan ÌýEVPfefferbaum ÌýAÌýCompromised white matter tract integrity in schizophrenia inferred from diffusion tensor imaging.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56367-Ìý374
64.
Lim ÌýKOAdalsteinsson ÌýESpielman ÌýDSullivan ÌýEVRosenbloom ÌýMJPfefferbaum ÌýAÌýProton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging of cortical gray and white matter in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55346-Ìý352
65.
Näätänen ÌýRÌýAttention and Brain Function.Ìý Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence A Erlbaum Associates1992;
66.
Oranje ÌýBvan Berckel ÌýBNMKemner ÌýCvan Ree ÌýJMKahn ÌýRSVerbaten ÌýMNÌýThe effects of a subanaethetic dose of ketamine on human selective attention.ÌýÌý±·±ð³Ü°ù´Ç±è²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²¹°ù³¾²¹³¦´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 2000;22293-Ìý302
67.
Alho ÌýKÌýCerebral generators of mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic counterpart (MMNm) elicited by sound changes.ÌýÌýEar Hear. 1995;1638-Ìý51
68.
Kropotov ÌýJDNäätänen ÌýRSevostianov ÌýAVAlho ÌýKReinikainen ÌýKKropotova ÌýOVÌýMismatch negativity to auditory stimulus change recorded directly from the human temporal cortex.ÌýÌý±Ê²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²â²õ¾±´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1995;32418-Ìý422
69.
Alain ÌýCWoods ÌýDLKnight ÌýRTÌýA distributed cortical network for auditory sensory memory in humans.ÌýÌýBrain Res. 1998;81223-Ìý37
70.
Alho ÌýKWoods ÌýDLAlgazi ÌýAKnight ÌýRTNäätänen ÌýRÌýLesions of frontal cortex diminish the auditory mismatch negativity.ÌýÌýElectroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1994;91353-Ìý362
71.
Born ÌýJBräuninger ÌýWFehm-Wolfsdorf ÌýGVoigt ÌýKPauschinger ÌýPFehm ÌýHLÌýDose-dependent influences on electrophysiological signs of attention in humans after neuropeptide ACTH 4-10.ÌýÌýExp Brain Res. 1987;6785-Ìý92
72.
Born ÌýJKern ÌýWPietrowsky ÌýRSittig ÌýWFehm ÌýHLÌýFragments of ACTH affect electrophysiological signs of controlled stimulus processing in humans.ÌýÌý±Ê²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²¹°ù³¾²¹³¦´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1989;99439-Ìý444
73.
Winsberg ÌýBGJavitt ÌýDCSilipo ÌýGSÌýElectrophysiological indices of information processing in methylphenidate responders.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1997;42434-Ìý445
74.
Jevtovic-Todorovic ÌýVTodorovic ÌýSMMennerick ÌýSPowell ÌýSDikranian ÌýKBenshoff ÌýNZorumski ÌýCFOlney ÌýJWÌýNitrous oxide (laughing gas) is an NMDA antagonist, neuroprotectant and neurotoxin.ÌýÌýNat Med. 1998;4460-Ìý463
75.
Mennerick ÌýSJevtovic-Todorovic ÌýVTodorovic ÌýSMShen ÌýWOlney ÌýJWZorumski ÌýCFÌýEffect of nitrous oxide on excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission in hippocampal cultures.ÌýÌýJ Neurosci. 1998;189716-Ìý9726
76.
Tsai ÌýGGastfriend ÌýDRCoyle ÌýJTÌýThe glutamatergic basis of human alcoholism.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1995;152332-Ìý340
77.
Jääskeläinen ÌýIPLehtokoski ÌýAAlho ÌýKKujala ÌýTPekkonen ÌýESinclair ÌýJDNäätänen ÌýRSillanaukee ÌýPÌýLow dose of ethanol suppresses mismatch negativity of auditory event-related potentials.ÌýÌýAlcohol Clin Exp Res. 1995;19607-Ìý610
78.
Jääskeläinen ÌýIPPekkonen ÌýEAlho ÌýKSinclair ÌýJDSillanaukee ÌýPNäätänen ÌýRÌýDose-related effect of alcohol on mismatch negativity and reaction time performance.ÌýÌý´¡±ô³¦´Ç³ó´Ç±ô. 1995;12491-Ìý495
79.
Jääskeläinen ÌýIPPekkonen ÌýEHirvonen ÌýJSillanaukee ÌýPNäätänen ÌýRÌýMismatch negativity subcomponents and ethyl alcohol.ÌýÌýBiol Psychol. 1996;4313-Ìý25
80.
Pang ÌýEWFowler ÌýBÌýDissociation of the mismatch negativity and processing negativity attentional waveforms with nitrous oxide.ÌýÌý±Ê²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²â²õ¾±´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1999;36552-Ìý558
81.
Oye ÌýIHustveit ÌýOMaurset ÌýARatti Moberg ÌýEPaulsen ÌýOSkoglund ÌýLAÌýThe chiral forms of ketamine as probes for NMDA-receptor function in humans.ÌýKameyama ÌýTNabeshima ÌýTDomino ÌýEFeds.ÌýNMDA Receptor Related Agents Biochemistry, Pharmacology and Behavior Ann Arbor, Mich NPP Books1991;381-Ìý389
82.
Hustveit ÌýOMaurset ÌýAOye ÌýIÌýInteraction of the chiral forms of ketamine with opioid, phencyclidine, sigma and muscarinic receptors.ÌýÌýPharmacol Toxicol. 1995;77355-Ìý359
83.
Moghaddam ÌýBAdams ÌýBVerma ÌýADaly ÌýDÌýActivation of glutamatergic neurotransmission by ketamine: a novel step in the pathway from NMDA receptor blockade to dopaminergic and cognitive disruptions associated with the prefrontal cortex.ÌýÌýJ Neurosci. 1997;172921-Ìý2927
84.
Moghaddam ÌýBAdams ÌýBWÌýReversal of phencyclidine effects by a group II metabotropic glutamate receptor agonist in rats.ÌýÌý³§³¦¾±±ð²Ô³¦±ð. 1998;2811349-Ìý1352
85.
Aghajanian ÌýGKMarek ÌýGJÌýSerotonin, via 5-HT2A receptors, increases EPSCs in layer V pyramidal cells of prefrontal cortex by an asynchronous mode of glutamate release.ÌýÌýBrain Res. 1999;825161-Ìý171
86.
Umbricht ÌýDVollenweider ÌýFXSchmid ÌýLKoller ÌýRÌýInvestigations into the neurobiology of information processing deficits in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýSchizophr Res. 2000;41290-Ìý291
87.
Umbricht ÌýDVollenweider ÌýFXÌýEffects of NMDA-antagonists and 5-HT2A-agonists on generation of MMN in human volunteers [abstract].ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1999;4551³§-Ìý52³§
Original Article
¶Ù±ð³¦±ð³¾²ú±ð°ùÌý2000

Ketamine-Induced Deficits in Auditory and Visual Context-Dependent Processing in Healthy Volunteers: Implications for Models of Cognitive Deficits in Schizophrenia

Author Affiliations

From the Department of Research, Psychiatric University Hospital, Zurich, Switzerland (Drs Umbricht, Vollenweider, and Hell and Mss Schmid and Koller); and Program in Cognitive Neuroscience and Schizophrenia, Nathan Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg, NY (Dr Javitt).

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57(12):1139-1147. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.57.12.1139
Abstract

BackgroundÌý In patients with schizophrenia, deficient generation of mismatch negativity (MMN)—an event-related potential (ERP) indexing auditory sensory ("echoic") memory—and a selective increase of "context dependent" ("BX") errors in the "A-X" version of the Continuous Performance Test (AX-CPT) indicate an impaired ability to form and use transient memory traces. Animal and human studies implicate deficient N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor (NMDAR) functioning in such abnormalities. In this study, effects of the NMDAR antagonists ketamine on MMN generation and AX-CPT performance were investigated in healthy volunteers to test the hypothesis that NMDARs are critically involved in human MMN generation, and to assess the nature of ketamine-induced deficits in AX-CPT performance.

MethodsÌý In a single-blind placebo-controlled study, 20 healthy volunteers underwent an infusion with subanesthetic doses of ketamine. The MMN-to-pitch and MMN-to-duration deviants were obtained while subjects performed an AX-CPT.

ResultsÌý Ketamine significantly decreased the peak amplitudes of the MMN-to-pitch and MMN-to-duration deviants by 27% and 21%, respectively. It induced performance deficits in the AX-CPT characterized by decreased hit rates and specific increases of errors (BX errors), reflecting a failure to form and use transient memory traces of task relevant information.

ConclusionsÌý The NMDARs are critically involved in human MMN generation. Deficient MMN in schizophrenia thus suggests deficits in NMDAR-related neurotransmission. N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor dysfunction may also contribute to the impairment of patients with schizophrenia in forming and using transient memory traces in more complex tasks, such as the AX-CPT. Thus, NMDAR-related dysfunction may underlie deficits in transient memory at different levels of information processing in schizophrenia.

NEUROCOGNITIVE deficits represent a significant feature of schizophrenia1-7 and an important limiting factor for functional outcome.8-10 They include a deficient ability to form and use transient memory traces of task relevant information. For example, patients with schizophrenia show deficits in auditory sensory ("echoic") memory, which encodes and maintains representations of simple physical features of auditory stimuli (eg, pitch, intensity) for up to 30 seconds after stimulus presentation.11 These deficits are manifested in an impaired ability to match tones after a brief delay12-14 and in deficient generation of an event-related potential (ERP) component that is termed mismatch negativity (MMN) and represents an index of echoic memory.5,15-21 Similarly, patients with schizophrenia perform poorly in more complex attention-dependent tasks—such as a modified version of the "A-X" Continuous Performance Test (AX-CPT)—that require the successful formation and transient maintenance of a representation of task relevant information for correct task performance.22,23

Deficient N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDAR)–dependent neurotransmission has been implicated in neurocognitive deficits of schizophrenia24,25 by similarities of cognitive deficits26-31 and abnormal brain activation patterns32-34 that are induced by NMDAR antagonists with abnormalities in schizophrenia. Deficient NDMAR functioning may specifically contribute to failures in transient memory, since NMDAR antagonists impair performance in tasks that tax transient maintenance of information.28,35-39 Furthermore, in nonhuman primates, NMDAR antagonists selectively abolish MMN, indicating a crucial role of NMDARs in MMN generation and echoic memory40,41 These results thus suggest that deficient NMDAR-dependent neurotransmission may underlie deficits in MMN generation and echoic memory in schizophrenia5,15-21 and possibly contribute to impaired performance in tasks in which transient maintenance of information is crucial, such as the AX-CPT.

This study tested the hypothesis that in healthy volunteers NMDAR blockade—induced by low-dose ketamine—results in a selective deficit in MMN generation to provide supporting or refuting evidence for the assumption that MMN deficits in schizophrenia may indeed reflect abnormal NMDAR functioning. In addition, ketamine-induced deficits in the performance of the AX-CPT were explored. Specifically, we were interested in their resemblance to the deficits observed in schizophrenia.21 We hypothesized that ketamine would significantly decrease MMN without reducing sensory event-related potentials (ERPs), such as N1, and specifically increase those errors in the AX-CPT that are indicative of deficient formation and use of transient memory traces.

Ketamine—a so-called dissociative anesthetic—is a noncompetitive NMDAR antagonist binding to the phencyclidine site within the ion channel of the NMDAR complex.25 Its well-documented psychotomimetic effects at subanesthetic doses26,42-45 are short-lived because of its short half-life.46 Ketamine anesthesia produces minimal cardiac and respiratory effects, is not associated with any long-term psychological effects,47 and shows an excellent safety record.48 The minimal risks associated with low-dose ketamine justify its use as a pharmacological probe in healthy volunteers if important questions concerning the basis of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia are addressed that cannot be answered by studies in animals. That is the case in this study.

Subjects and methods

The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Psychiatric University Hospital Zurich and conducted in the Research Department of the Psychiatric University Hospital.

Subjects

Normal subjects were recruited in the local university and technical college through advertisement. During the initial screening interview by the principal investigator (D.U.), the rationale and the goal of the study were explained in lay terms to the subjects. In addition, the expected psychotomimetic and dissociative effects were reviewed in detail and described as accurately as possible. Potential physical adverse effects of low-dose ketamine, such as nausea, vomiting, and dizziness, were discussed with all subjects. Subjects were informed that they would be under constant supervision during the whole session and that the experiment would be stopped at any time if they wished or if adverse effects occurred. All potential study candidates were thus fully informed about the goal and the risks of the study orally and in writing, and signed informed consent. After signing informed consent, subjects underwent a screening that included a structured clinical interview (Composite International Clinical Interview49) assessing present and past personal psychiatric history, a semistructured interview covering family psychiatric history, a physical examination, and laboratory tests that included a complete blood cell count, routine blood chemistry, and an electrocardiogram. Exclusion criteria were a history of Axis I disorders, a history of drug dependence or present drug abuse, a psychiatric family history of Axis I disorders extending to second-degree relatives, and the presence of any medical disorders. None of the female subjects was pregnant. Intelligence was assessed using the Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligence Scale, Revised Version.50 Handedness was determined using the handedness scale by Chapman and Chapman.51 Twenty-one subjects were enrolled in the study. They received a financial remuneration for participation in this study. One female subject was dropped from the study after she developed nausea and vomiting during the ketamine infusion, which necessitated a premature stop of test procedures. Thus, 20 subjects (14 men and 6 women) were included in the final analysis. Their mean + SD age was 24.6 + 2.9 years. Eighteen were university students; 2 had completed an apprenticeship and were employed at the time of the study. Nineteen of the subjects were right-handed; the mean + SD verbal and performance IQ scores were 114 + 11 and 117 + 9, respectively.

Procedures
Ketamine/Placebo Infusion

Each subject underwent both a placebo and ketamine infusion on 2 separate days in a randomized and counter-balanced order. Subjects were blind to infusion order. On both infusion days, subjects arrived in the laboratory around 9 AM after an overnight fast. An indwelling catheter was placed in the antecubital vein of the nondominant arm and an infusion of physiological sodium chloride solution and 5% glucose was started and kept at a flow of 20 mL/h. Then, a baseline ERP recording was acquired using the test paradigms described below. After the baseline phase, a bolus of ketamine, 0.24 mg/kg, was given intravenously over 5 minutes, followed by a 5-minute pause. Then, a maintenance infusion of ketamine, 0.9 mg/kg per hour, was started. Previous research has demonstrated that ketamine blood levels slowly increase during a constant ketamine infusion.52 Thus, to keep ketamine levels fairly constant, the dose was reduced by 10% every 15 minutes. In the placebo session, the same schedule (bolus, pause, and maintenance) was followed. Instead of ketamine, an infusion of physiological sodium chloride solution and 5% glucose was given. Twenty minutes after the start of the ketamine or placebo bolus the second ERP recording began. On completion of the second ERP recording, the ketamine or placebo infusion was stopped. Subjects were given 30 minutes to recover from the ketamine or placebo infusion; then a third ERP recording was acquired. During the entire session, subjects were under the constant supervision of study personnel.

Behavioral Ratings

At the end of each ERP recording session, behavioral effects were assessed using the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) (scale points, 1-7).53 In 1 subject, several items could not be reliably assessed during ketamine administration; therefore, for some analyses, only data from 19 subjects were available. Orientation to person, place, and time were assessed with 3 items from the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination54 (date and place of birth, temporal orientation, and spatial orientation) that yield a maximum score of 25 indicating full orientation. This measure was obtained in 18 subjects.

Auditory Test Paradigm

The ERP recordings were acquired during the presentation of auditory stimuli, consisting of 100-millisecond, 1000-Hz standard intermixed with 100-millisecond, 1500-Hz pitch deviants and 250-millisecond, 1000-Hz duration deviants. Stimuli were presented in a fixed order (9 standard, 1 pitch deviant, 9 standard, and 1 duration deviant) with an stimulus onset asynchrony of 300 milliseconds and presented through foam insert earphones at nominal intensity of 75-dB sound pressure level. Stimuli were presented in 4 blocks with 1517 stimuli each. During presentation of the auditory test paradigm, subjects performed a visual AX-CPT to minimize their attention to the auditory stimuli.

The AX-CPT

Visual stimuli consisted of individual letters and were presented sequentially on a computer screen for 250 milliseconds each. Subjects were instructed to press a button whenever the letter A ("correct cue") was followed by the letter X ("correct target"). All other sequences were to be ignored, including sequences in which an incorrect cue (designated "B," but consisting of all letters other than A or X) was followed by the target letter (X) or sequences in which a correct cue (A) was followed by an incorrect target (designated "Y," but consisting of all letters other than A or X). Stimuli were presented in 4 blocks of 280 stimuli (140 pairs) each. Within each block, 50% of the cue-target sequences were presented with short interstimulus interval (ISI) (0.8 seconds) and 50% with long ISI (4 seconds). Short and long ISI were pseudorandomly intermixed. The time between stimulus pairs was constant at 0.8 seconds. Seventy percent of the stimulus pairs were AX sequences; all other sequences (BX, AY, and BY) occurred with a probability of .1 each.

ERP Recordings

Electroencephalogram recordings were acquired with a Neuroscan SCAN system (Neuroscan Labs, Sterling, Va) and obtained from 28 scalp locations, consisting of standard 10/20 placements plus right and left mastoid placement along with right vertical and horizontal electro-oculographic electrodes. An electrode placed on the nose served as reference. Electrode impedance was kept below 5 kΩ. Activity was amplified with a band pass of 0.1 to 50 Hz (6 dB down) and digitized continuously at a sampling rate of 500 Hz. Digital tags were obtained to all auditory stimuli.

Epochs were constructed that consisted of a 100-millisecond prestimulus baseline and a 500-millisecond poststimulus interval. After correction of vertical eye movements and blinks using the algorithm provided by Neuroscan software, epochs with amplitudes exceeding ±75 µV at any electrode except the vertical eye leads were excluded from averaging. After artifact rejection, epochs were averaged offline for each subject, session, phase, and stimulus type, and digitally filtered with a low-pass filter of 15 Hz (24 dB down).

Analyses focused on generation of N1 and P2 to the standard stimuli and MMN generation to pitch- and duration-deviant stimuli. Mismatch negativity waveforms were obtained by subtracting waveforms elicited by standards from waveforms elicited by pitch- or duration-deviant stimuli. Waveforms were mathematically referenced to an average mastoid reference before peak detection. For N1, amplitude was defined as peak negativity within the 50- to 150-millisecond latency; for P2, amplitude was defined as peak positivity within the 150- to 250-millisecond latency. For pitch MMN, amplitude was defined as the peak negativity within the 100- to 225-millisecond latency window. For duration MMN, amplitude was defined as the peak negativity within the 200- to 300-millisecond range.

For all subjects and recording phases, the number of sweeps available for computing the MMN average waves exceeded a threshold of 100 that was set because of signal-to-noise concerns. In addition, the number of sweeps that survived artifact rejection and were included in the MMN average waves did not differ significantly for each phase of both infusions (mean [SD] number of averaged sweeps in all phases) (for standard stimuli, 3741 [756]; for pitch deviants, 235 [46]; and for duration deviants, 236 [47]).

Statistical analysis

For ERP analyses, the primary dependent measures consisted of amplitude and latency of N1 and P2 at Fz to the standard stimulus and amplitude and latency of pitch and duration MMN at Fz. Effects of ketamine were evaluated using repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with infusion (ketamine vs placebo) and time (preinfusion baseline phase vs ketamine/placebo infusion phase vs postinfusion phase) as repeated measures. For the analysis of MMN, an additional within-subject 2-level factor denoting MMN type (pitch vs duration condition) was included. Paired t tests were performed for post hoc analysis if indicated. Additional topographic analyses evaluated the effects of ketamine administration on MMN distribution across relevant electrode pairs.

For the analyses of performance of AX-CPT, dependent measures consisted of percentage correct detection of correct cue/correct target ("AX") sequences (hit rate), and percentage of incorrect responses (false alarms) to other ("BX," "AY," and "BY") sequences. Effects of ketamine on hit rate were evaluated with 2 × 3 × 2-factorial repeated-measures ANOVAs with infusion (ketamine vs placebo); time (preinfusion baseline phase vs ketamine/placebo infusion phase vs postinfusion phase); and ISI (short vs long) as repeated measures. Analyses of false-alarm rates were done similarly with an additional within-subject factor with 3 levels denoting false-alarm type (BX vs AY vs BY). Differences between rates of BX errors and AY and BY errors, respectively, during the 3 phases of both sessions were evaluated with simple within-subject contrasts involving infusion (contrast: ketamine vs placebo); time (contrasts: preinfusion baseline phase vs ketamine/placebo infusion phase; preinfusion baseline phase vs postinfusion phase); and false-alarm type (contrasts: BX vs AY and BX vs BY). Post hoc paired t tests were used to assess specific differences if indicated.

For behavioral analyses, we used the BPRS total score and 5 factors that can be derived from the BPRS items as follows: (1) psychosis factor: conceptual disorganization, suspiciousness, hallucinatory behavior, and unusual thought content; (2) activation factor: tension, mannerism and posturing, and excitement; (3) anxiety/depression factor: somatic concern, anxiety, guilt feelings, and depressive mood; (4) anergia factor: emotional withdrawal, motor retardation, and blunted affect; and (5) hostility factor: hostility, suspiciousness, and uncooperativeness. Effects of ketamine were evaluated with 2 × 3-factorial repeated-measures ANOVAs with infusion (ketamine vs placebo) and time (preinfusion baseline phase vs ketamine/placebo infusion phase vs postinfusion phase) as repeated measures.

For all statistical analyses, a level of α = .05 was considered significant.

Results
Effects of ketamine on behavior and orientation

During ketamine administration, the expected behavioral changes were observed: The mean + SD BPRS total score increased from 19.1 + 1.3 to 33.3 + 8.2 (infusion × time interaction: F2,18 = 31.32, P<.001). In addition, significant increases of the mean + SD BPRS psychosis factor scores (baseline: 4.0 + 0.0; ketamine administration: 9.8 + 3.8; and infusion × time interaction: F2,18 = 22.72, P<.001) and the mean + SD BPRS anergia factor scores (baseline: 3.5 + 0.8; ketamine administration: 8.5 + 3.7; and infusion × time interaction: F2,18 = 18.37, P<.001) were observed. There were also significant, but less pronounced, increases of the mean + SD BPRS anxiety/depression factor (baseline: 4.3 + 0.7; ketamine administration: 5.7 + 2.5; and infusion × time interaction: F2,18 = 4.00, P = .04) and activation factor scores (baseline: 3.2 + 0.6; ketamine administration: 4.3 + 2.5; and infusion × time interaction: F2,18 = 3.61, P = .05) during ketamine administration. Ketamine did not affect autobiographic, temporal, or spatial orientation of subjects (repeated-measures ANOVA for ketamine infusion: F1,17 = 1.76, P = .20). In 1 subject, there was a decrease by 6 points, and in 2 subjects, a decrease by 1 point of the total score were noted. In all other subjects, orientation scores remained unchanged.

EFFECTS OF KETAMINE ON AUDITORY ERPs
Mismatch Negativity

In both baseline sessions, robust MMN amplitudes to pitch and duration deviants were recorded with MMN maxima at Fz. As expected, MMN latency to pitch-duration–deviant stimuli were approximately 100 milliseconds longer than to pitch-deviant stimuli in the baseline phases of both the placebo and ketamine session (Table 1) (Figure 1).

Ketamine administration was associated with a significant reduction of MMN peak amplitudes in the pitch- and duration-deviance condition (Table 2) (Figure 2). During ketamine administration, the mean peak amplitude of MMN-to-pitch deviants decreased by 1.47 µV, or 27%; the mean peak amplitude of MMN-to-duration deviants decreased by 0.9 µV, or 21%, from the corresponding baseline values of that session. A repeated-measures ANOVA demonstrated a significant infusion × time interaction (F2,18 = 4.57, P = .03). The infusion × time × MMN-type interaction was not significant, indicating that the effect of ketamine on MMN-to-pitch and MMN-to-duration deviants was of similar magnitude. After ketamine administration, MMN in the pitch condition recovered partially to the session baseline value; in the duration condition, MMN remained reduced. Repeated-measures ANOVAs of MMN performed separately for each condition confirmed significant infusion × time interactions (pitch condition: F2,18 = 4.42, P = .03; duration condition: F2,18 = 4.86, P = .02). Post hoc paired t tests confirmed a significantly smaller MMN during ketamine administration than during the session baseline (pitch condition: t19 = −4.82, P<.001; duration condition: t19 = −2.19, P = .04). The corresponding comparisons for the placebo session were not significant.

Ketamine administration was associated with a slight, but significant increase of MMN peak latencies (infusion × time interaction: F2,18 = 9.96, P<.001) mainly as a result of an increase of the peak latency of MMN in the duration condition. Post hoc paired t tests demonstrated significantly longer MMN peak latency-to-duration deviants during ketamine administration than at session baseline (t19 = 2.41, P = .03) and than during placebo administration (t19 = 3.56, P = .002).

MMN Topography

Further analyses investigated effects of ketamine administration on MMN at specific electrode pairs (C3/C4, T3/T4, and P3/P4) that have been reported to be associated with lateralized dysfunction in schizophrenia.55 For each electrode pair, infusion × time × hemisphere interactions were not significant, indicating that the ketamine administration was not associated with a different left-right topography.

N1/P2 Amplitudes and Latencies

During the baseline of both sessions, subjects showed amplitudes and latencies of N1 and P2 in the range expected for an stimulus onset asynchrony of 300 milliseconds (Table 2). Ketamine administration was associated with a small, but significant, increase of N1 peak amplitude, resulting in a significant infusion × time interaction (F2,18 = 4.70, P = .02). Post hoc paired t tests demonstrated a significantly larger N1 peak amplitude during ketamine than placebo administration (t19 = −3,69, P = .002). The increase of N1 amplitude during ketamine administration from the session baseline failed to reach significance (post hoc paired t test: t19 = 1,88, P = .07). Ketamine administration did not affect N1 peak latency.

The P2 amplitude increased slightly, but significantly, throughout both recording sessions (effect of time: F2,18 = 16.01, P<.001). Ketamine administration was not associated with any particular effect on peak amplitude (infusion × time interaction: F2,18 = 0.89, P = .40). There was no significant effect on peak latency of P2.

Effects of ketamine on ax-cpt performance

During the collection of MMN data, subjects performed the AX-CPT (1 subject completely stopped performing the task during ketamine administration; thus, data of 19 subjects were included in the analysis). Ketamine administration was associated with a significant decline of the correct detection of the AX-sequence (hit rate) at both ISIs (Figure 2 and Figure 3). A repeated-measures ANOVA demonstrated a significant infusion × time interaction (F2,17 = 10.02, P = .001). The infusion × time × ISI interaction was not significant, indicating no differential impairment of performance at the 2 ISIs (F2,17 = 2.19, P = .14). Post hoc paired t tests confirmed significantly lower hit rates during ketamine administration than during the baseline of the ketamine infusion (short ISI: t18 = −4.63, P<.001; long ISI: t18 = −4.60, P<.001). Ketamine administration was associated with an increase of false alarms that was most pronounced for false alarms to BX sequences at both short and long ISI. A repeated-measures ANOVA demonstrated a significant 3-way interaction of infusion × time × false-alarm type (F4,15 = 3.33, P = .04). The degree of false-alarm increases were similar at short and long ISI, so that the infusion type × false-alarm type × ISI effect was nonsignificant (F4,15 = 1.61, P = .37). Contrasting BX error rates with rates of AY and BY errors, a significantly greater increase of BX errors than both AY and BY errors, was observed during ketamine administration as compared with baseline and placebo administration (contrasts: ketamine vs placebo session, baseline vs infusion phase, and BX vs AY error rates: F1,18 = 5.54, P = .03; contrasts: ketamine vs placebo session, baseline vs infusion phase, and BX vs BY error rates: F1,18 = 12.14, P = .003). The BX error rates were not significantly different than AY or BY error rates during the postinfusion phase as compared with the baseline phase (contrasts: ketamine vs placebo session, baseline vs postinfusion phase, BX vs AY error rates: F1,18 = 0.22, P = .60; contrasts: ketamine vs placebo session, baseline vs postinfusion phase, BX vs BY error rates: F1,18 = 0.05, P = .80). Paired t tests confirmed significantly higher BX error rates during ketamine administration than during baseline of the ketamine session for both ISIs (short ISI: t18 = −4.08, P = .001; long ISI: t18 = −3.55, P = .005).

Similar results were obtained, when a′ measures obtained from signal-detection analyses and calculated separately for both ISIs, were used in place of BX false-alarm rates. There was a significant infusion × time interaction: F2,17 = 21.14, P<.001), but a nonsignificant infusion × time × ISI interaction: (F2,17 = 1.21, P = .30), again indicating a significantly worse performance during ketamine administration irrespective of ISI. Paired t tests demonstrated a significant decline of a′ at short and long ISI during ketamine administration as compared with baseline values (short ISI: t18 = −6.24. P<.001; long ISI: t18 = −6.18, P<.001).

Comment

This study evaluated the hypothesis that NMDARs are critically involved in formation and use of transient memory traces, both at the auditory sensory level and in a task engaging prefrontal circuitry.56 Specifically, the hypothesis was tested that, in humans, MMN generation—an index of the auditory sensory memory—depends on intact NMDAR functioning. The results support this hypothesis: In healthy volunteers, subanesthetic doses of the NMDA antagonist ketamine significantly diminished MMN generation, both in a pitch and a duration deviance condition, without reducing sensory ERPs with similar latency. Thus, the reduction of MMN is not caused by a general weakening of ERP generators, but must specifically involve the neuronal processes generating the MMN wave. Furthermore, the expected latency shift of approximately 100 milliseconds between MMN-to-pitch deviants and MMN-to-duration deviants was observed throughout both recording sessions and thus served as additional validation criterion that the ERP components affected by ketamine were indeed MMN waves.

The ability of ketamine to inhibit MMN generation in humans is consistent with the reduction of MMN in monkeys after infusion of NMDA antagonists into primary auditory cortex.40,41,57 The accompanying article by Javitt et al21 demonstrates impaired MMN generation, both in a pitch and duration deviance condition in patients with recent-onset and chronic schizophrenia, confirming previous reports of MMN deficits in schizophrenia.5,15-20 Our findings thus suggest that these deficits may be direct manifestations of deficient NMDAR-dependent neurotransmission. Reduced glutamate release,58 abnormal expression of specific subunits of the NMDAR,59-61 reduced messenger RNA for specific subunits of the NMDAR,62 and evidence for altered connectivity63,64 have been described in schizophrenia and could possibly lead or contribute to NMDA/glutamatergic hypofunction. Our results thus provide indirect evidence in support of the glutamate/NMDA hypothesis of schizophrenia

In our study, generally greater MMN amplitudes and larger MMN-to-pitch than MMN-to-duration deviants were observed compared with the study by Javitt et al.21 These differences are consistent with an association of greater MMN with a lower probability of the deviant stimuli and a larger difference between pitch deviant and standard stimulus, respectively, as used in our study.65 Also, ketamine increased the latency of MMN-to-duration deviants, while this latency was not abnormal in patients with schizophrenia in the study by Javitt et al. This disparity may be related to the lower deviant probability in our study, but may also point to real differences between ketamine-induced MMN deficits and those observed in schizophrenia.

Our results are at odds with a recent study in which ketamine reduced ERP measures of selective attention, but not MMN.66 However, this study used a much smaller dose of ketamine. Second, the paradigm used in this study is suited for investigating ERP indices of selective attention, but not MMN. In addition, low numbers of deviant stimuli as used in the paradigm of that study result in a poor signal-to-noise ratio, further obscuring subtle but potentially significant changes.

Generators for MMN have been localized to primary auditory cortex.57,67,68 Consistently, infusion of NMDA antagonists into primary auditory cortex of monkeys abolishes MMN.40 Thus, it is parsimonious to assume that ketamine exerted its main effect on MMN generation locally in these cortical areas. However, in humans prefrontal lesions are also associated with deficits in MMN generation and, interestingly, slight increases of N1,69,70 similar to the ketamine-induced effects in the present study. Thus, early sensory processing failures may be caused, in part, by a loss of top-down control. Ketamine induces a marked increase of metabolic activity in prefrontal areas.32-34 Therefore, it is conceivable that a loss of this top-down control secondary to ketamine-induced prefrontal dysfunction exerted an additional reducing effect on MMN generation and was responsible for the slight increase of N1.

This study also investigated ketamine-induced performance deficits in a modified AX-CPT. Ketamine administration not only decreased hit rates significantly, but was associated with a specific and significant increase of those errors that are assumed to reflect a failure to form and use a transient representation of a cue stimulus (BX errors). In contrast, ketamine did not significantly increase the rates of other possible errors where such a transient memory is not tapped (AY and BY errors). Thus, higher BX errors cannot be ascribed to an indiscriminate increase in errors by subjects. This deficit did not disproportionally worsen at the long ISI, indicating that ketamine did not impair the maintenance of the memory trace (ie, result in a faster decay of information). Rather, the profile of deficits is consistent with an impairment of processes that may involve or affect the initial formation and/or utilization of such a memory trace. This specific pattern closely resembles the pattern of deficient performance that schizophrenic patients show in this task: the accompanying publication by Javitt et al,21 and other studies of patients with schizophrenia using almost identical versions of the AX-CPT, similarly reported specific increases of context-dependent BX errors in patients.22,23,56 Only one study found an abnormal decay of information in schizophrenia.22 Thus, patients with schizophrenia mostly demonstrate deficits consistent with impaired formation and/or use of a memory trace in the absence of a pronounced abnormality in information maintenance The similarity of ketamine-induced deficits thus suggests that NMDAR-related dysfunction may contribute to the observed deficits in schizophrenia.

Are the effects of ketamine observed in this study specifically caused by its NMDA antagonist properties or would other agents affecting attention but not active at NMDAR exert similar effects? The available evidence argues against the latter: administration of a fragment of the adrenocorticotropic hormone resulted in a significant reduction of an ERP measure of selective attention without affecting MMN.71,72 Also, the stimulant methylphenidate did not affect MMN generation in hyperactive children despite a significant enhancement of attention-dependent ERPs.73 Furthermore, both subanesthetic doses of nitrous oxide—recently shown to be an NMDA antagonist74,75—and ethanol, which possesses NMDA antagonistic properties76—significantly reduce MMN without affecting ERP measures of selective attention.77-80 Ketamine used in this study is a racemic mixture of the enantiomeres S- and R-ketamine, with S-ketamine binding with a 4 to 5 times higher affinity to the phencyclidine-binding site of the NMDAR complex than R-ketamine.81 However, the affinity of both enantiomeres to the NMDAR is several-fold higher than their respective affinities for sigma, opioid, and muscarinic receptors.81,82 It is thus unlikely that direct actions at other receptors contributed substantially to the observed effects.

The NMDA antagonists induce excessive glutamate release83,84—an effect shared by 5-HT2A agonists85; thus, the reduction of MMN could have been the result of such an effect. However, in a recently completed study of 18 healthy volunteers the psychotomimetic psilocybin, a 5-HT2A agonist, did not significantly reduce MMN despite a comparable magnitude of behavioral effects. However, psilocybin induced similar deficits in AX-CPT performance.86,87 It is thus likely that ketamine-induced excessive glutamate release contributed substantially to the specific performance deficits in the AX-CPT.

Conclusions

This study demonstrated that the NMDAR antagonist ketamine induces deficits in MMN generation and performance of the AX-CPT that closely resemble deficits observed in schizophrenia. In both paradigms, the effects of NMDA blockade are consistent with impairments in the formation and use of transient memory traces. Deficient NMDAR-dependent neurotransmission may contribute to such deficits in schizophrenia. This study thus supports the glutamatergic/NMDA model of schizophrenia.

Accepted for publication September 30, 1999.

Supported by grant SNF 32-050957.97 from the Swiss National Science Foundation, Berne, Switzerland (Dr Umbricht).

Presented in part at the 7th International Congress on Schizophrenia Research, Santa Fe, NM, April 1-21, 1999.

Corresponding author: Daniel Umbricht, MD, Psychiatric University Hospital, Department of Research, PO Box 68, Zurich 8029, Switzerland (e-mail: umbricht@bli.unizh.ch).

References
1.
Bilder ÌýRMÌýThe neuropsychology of schizophrenia: what, when, where, how?ÌýFleischhacker ÌýWWHinterhuber ÌýHMeise ÌýUeds.ÌýSchizophrene Störungen State of the Art II, Ursachen-Behandlung-Verlauf Innsbruck, Switzerland Verlag Integrative Psychiatrie1998;155-Ìý171
2.
Pantelis ÌýCNelson ÌýHEBarnes ÌýTREÌýSchizophrenia: A Neuropsychological Perspective.Ìý New York, NY John Wiley & Sons Inc1998;
3.
Cornblatt ÌýBAKeilp ÌýJGÌýImpaired attention, genetics, and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.ÌýÌýSchizophr Bull. 1994;2031-Ìý46
4.
Pritchard ÌýWSÌýCognitive event-related potential correlates of schizophrenia.ÌýÌýPsychol Bull. 1986;10043-Ìý66
5.
Javitt ÌýDCDoneshka ÌýPGrochowski ÌýSRitter ÌýWÌýImpaired mismatch negativity generation reflects widespread dysfunction of working memory in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52550-Ìý558
6.
Braff ÌýDLGeyer ÌýMAÌýSensorimotor gating and schizophrenia: human and animal model studies.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47181-Ìý188
7.
Braff ÌýDLGrillon ÌýCGeyer ÌýMAÌýGating and habituation of the startle reflex in schizophrenic patients.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49206-Ìý215
8.
Green ÌýMFÌýWhat are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia?ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1996;153321-Ìý330
9.
Bilder ÌýRMÌýNeurocognitive impairment in schizophrenia and how it affects treatment options.ÌýÌýCan J Psychiatry. 1997;42255-Ìý264
10.
Goldman ÌýRSAxelrod ÌýBNTandon ÌýRRibeiro ÌýSCCraig ÌýKBerent ÌýSÌýNeuropsychological prediction of treatment efficacy and one-year outcome in schizophrenia.ÌýÌý±Ê²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è²¹³Ù³ó´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1993;26122-Ìý126
11.
Lu ÌýZLWilliamson ÌýSJKaufman ÌýLÌýBehavioral lifetime of human auditory sensory memory predicted by physiological measures.ÌýÌý³§³¦¾±±ð²Ô³¦±ð. 1992;2581668-Ìý1670
12.
Strous ÌýRDCowan ÌýNRitter ÌýWJavitt ÌýDCÌýAuditory sensory ("echoic") memory dysfunction in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1995;1521517-Ìý1519
13.
Javitt ÌýDCStrous ÌýRDGrochowski ÌýSRitter ÌýWCowan ÌýNÌýImpaired precision, but normal retention, of auditory sensory ("echoic") memory information in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýJ Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106315-Ìý324
14.
Wexler ÌýBEStevens ÌýAABowers ÌýAASernyak ÌýMJGoldman-Rakic ÌýPSÌýWord and tone working memory deficits in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;551093-Ìý1096
15.
Catts ÌýSVÌýBrain potential evidence for an auditory sensory memory deficit in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1995;152213-Ìý219
16.
Javitt ÌýDCDoneshka ÌýPZylberman ÌýIRitter ÌýWVaughan ÌýHG ÌýJrÌýImpairment of early cortical processing in schizophrenia: an event-related potential confirmation study.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1993;33513-Ìý519
17.
Oades ÌýRDZerbin ÌýDEggers ÌýCÌýStimulus-Vergleichsprozesse bei psychotischen Jugendlichen mit paranoiden und nicht paranoiden Symptomen: "Mismatch Negativity" deutet auf differenzierte Beeinträchtigungen hin.ÌýBaumann ÌýPed.ÌýBiologische Psychiatrie der Gegenwart New York, NY Springer-Verlag NY Inc1993;69-Ìý74
18.
Shelley ÌýAMWard ÌýPBCatts ÌýSVMichie ÌýPTAndrews ÌýSMcConaghy ÌýNÌýMismatch negativity: an index of a preattentive processing deficit in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1991;301059-Ìý1062
19.
Umbricht ÌýDJavitt ÌýDCNovak ÌýGPBates ÌýJAPollack ÌýSLieberman ÌýJKane ÌýJÌýEffects of clozapine on auditory event-related potentials in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1998;44716-Ìý725
20.
Umbricht ÌýDJavitt ÌýDCNovak ÌýGPBates ÌýJAPollak ÌýSLieberman ÌýJKane ÌýJÌýEffects of risperidone on auditory event-related potentials in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýInt J Neuropsychopharmacol. 1999;2229-Ìý304
21.
Javitt ÌýDCShelley ÌýAMSilipo ÌýGLieberman ÌýJAÌýDeficits in auditory and visual context-dependent process ing in schizophrenia: defining the pattern.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;571131-Ìý1137
22.
Servan-Schreiber ÌýDCohen ÌýJDSteingard ÌýSÌýSchizophrenic deficits in the processing of context: a test of a theoretical model.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;531105-Ìý1112
23.
Cohen ÌýJDBarch ÌýDMCarter ÌýCServan-Schreiber ÌýDÌýContext-processing deficits in schizophrenia: converging evidence from three theoretically motivated cognitive tasks.ÌýÌýJ Abnorm Psychol. 1999;108120-Ìý133
24.
Olney ÌýJWFarber ÌýNBÌýGlutamate receptor dysfunction and schizophrenia.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52998-Ìý1007
25.
Javitt ÌýDCZukin ÌýSRÌýRecent advances in the phencyclidine model of schizophrenia.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1991;1481301-Ìý1308
26.
Krystal ÌýJHKarper ÌýLPSeibyl ÌýJPFreeman ÌýGKDelaney ÌýRBremner ÌýJDHeninger ÌýGRBowers ÌýMB ÌýJrCharney ÌýDSÌýSubanesthetic effects of the noncompetitive NMDA antagonist, ketamine, in humans: psychotomimetic, perceptual, cognitive, and neuroendocrine responses.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51199-Ìý214
27.
Adler ÌýCMMalhotra ÌýAKElman ÌýIGoldberg ÌýTEgan ÌýMPickar ÌýDBreier ÌýAÌýComparison of ketamine-induced thought disorder in healthy volunteers and thought disorder in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1999;1561646-Ìý1649
28.
Adler ÌýCMGoldberg ÌýTEMalhotra ÌýAKPickar ÌýDBreier ÌýAÌýEffects of ketamine on thought disorder, working memory, and semantic memory in healthy volunteers.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1998;43811-Ìý816
29.
Bakshi ÌýVPSwerdlow ÌýNRGeyer ÌýMAÌýClozapine antagonizes phencyclidine-induced deficits in sensorimotor gating of the startle response.ÌýÌýJ Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1994;271787-Ìý794
30.
Bakshi ÌýVPGeyer ÌýMAÌýAntagonism of phencyclidine-induced deficits in prepulse inhibition by the putative atypical antipsychotic olanzapine.ÌýÌý±Ê²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²¹°ù³¾²¹³¦´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1995;122198-Ìý201
31.
Swerdlow ÌýNRBakshi ÌýVGeyer ÌýMAÌýSeroquel restores sensorimotor gating in phencyclidine-treated rats.ÌýÌýJ Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1996;2791290-Ìý1299
32.
Breier ÌýAMalhotra ÌýAKPinals ÌýDAWeisenfeld ÌýNIPickar ÌýDÌýAssociation of ketamine-induced psychosis with focal activation of the prefrontal cortex in healthy volunteers.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1997;154805-Ìý811
33.
Vollenweider ÌýFXLeenders ÌýKLOye ÌýIHell ÌýDAngst ÌýJÌýDifferential psychopathology and patterns of cerebral glucose utilisation produced by (S)- and (R)-ketamine in healthy volunteers using positron emission tomography (PET).ÌýÌýEur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1997;725-Ìý38
34.
Vollenweider ÌýFXLeenders ÌýKLScharfetter ÌýCAntonini ÌýAMaguire ÌýPMissimer ÌýJAngst ÌýJÌýMetabolic hyperfrontality and psychopathology in the ketamine model of psychosis using positron emission tomography (PET) and [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG).ÌýÌýEur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1997;79-Ìý24
35.
Ohno ÌýMYamamoto ÌýTWatanabe ÌýSÌýIntrahippocampal administration of a glycine site antagonist impairs working memory performance of rats.ÌýÌýEur J Pharmacol. 1994;253183-Ìý187
36.
Aura ÌýJRiekkinen ÌýP ÌýJrÌýBlockade of NMDA receptors located at the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory in rats.ÌýÌý±·±ð³Ü°ù´Ç°ù±ð±è´Ç°ù³Ù. 1999;10243-Ìý248
37.
Li ÌýHBMatsumoto ÌýKYamamoto ÌýMWatanabe ÌýHÌýNMDA but not AMPA receptor antagonists impair the delay-interposed radial maze performance of rats.ÌýÌýPharmacol Biochem Behav. 1997;58249-Ìý253
38.
Ohno ÌýMYamamoto ÌýTWatanabe ÌýSÌýEffects of intrahippocampal injections of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonists and scopolamine on working and reference memory assessed in rats by a three-panel runway task.ÌýÌýJ Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1992;263943-Ìý950
39.
Ohno ÌýMWatanabe ÌýSÌýEnhanced N-methyl-D-aspartate function reverses working memory failure induced by blockade of group I metabotropic glutamate receptors in the rat hippocampus.ÌýÌýNeurosci Lett. 1998;24037-Ìý40
40.
Javitt ÌýDCSteinschneider ÌýMSchroeder ÌýCEVaughan ÌýHG ÌýJrArezzo ÌýJCÌýDetection of stimulus deviance within primate primary auditory cortex: intracortical mechanisms of mismatch negativity (MMN) generation.ÌýÌýBrain Res. 1994;667192-Ìý200
41.
Javitt ÌýDCSteinschneider ÌýMSchroeder ÌýCEArezzo ÌýJCÌýRole of cortical N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in auditory sensory memory and mismatch negativity generation: implications for schizophrenia.ÌýÌýProc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;9311962-Ìý11967
42.
Bakker ÌýCBAmini ÌýFBÌýObservations on the psychomimetic effects of Sernyl.ÌýÌýComp Psychiatry. 1961;2269-Ìý280
43.
Ban ÌýTALohrenz ÌýJJLehmann ÌýHEÌýObservations on the action of Sernyl: a new psychotropic drug.ÌýÌýCan Psychiat Assoc J. 1961;6150-Ìý157
44.
Luby ÌýEDCohen ÌýBDRosenbaum ÌýGGottlieb ÌýJSKelley ÌýRÌýStudy of a new schizophrenomimetic drug: Sernyl.ÌýÌýArch Neurol Psychiatry. 1959;81363-Ìý369
45.
Malhotra ÌýAKPinals ÌýDAWeingartner ÌýHSirocco ÌýKMissar ÌýCDPickar ÌýDBreier ÌýAÌýNMDA receptor function and human cognition: the effects of ketamine in healthy volunteers.ÌýÌý±·±ð³Ü°ù´Ç±è²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²¹°ù³¾²¹³¦´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1996;14301-Ìý307
46.
Wieber ÌýJGugler ÌýRHengstmann ÌýJHDengler ÌýHJÌýPharmacokinetics of ketamine in man.ÌýÌý´¡²Ô²¹±ð²õ³Ù³ó±ð²õ¾±²õ³Ù. 1975;24260-Ìý263
47.
Domino ÌýEFChodoff ÌýPCorssen ÌýSÌýPharmacological effects of Cl-581, a new dissociative anesthetic in man.ÌýÌýClin Pharmacol Ther. 1965;6279-Ìý291
48.
Reich ÌýDLSilvay ÌýGÌýKetamine: an update on the first twenty-five years of clinical experience.ÌýÌýCan J Anaesth. 1989;36186-Ìý197
49.
Wittchen ÌýHSemler ÌýGÌýComposite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI): Beltz-Test, GmbH.Ìý Weinheim, Germany Psychologie Verlags Union1996;
50.
Tewes ÌýUÌýHAWIE-R: Hamburg-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Erwachsene, Revision 1991.Ìý2nd ed. Bern, Germany Hans Huber AG1994;
51.
Chapman ÌýLJChapman ÌýJPÌýThe measurement of handedness.ÌýÌýBrain Cogn. 1987;6175-Ìý183
52.
Feng ÌýNVollenweider ÌýFXMinder ÌýEIRentsch ÌýKGrampp ÌýTVonderschmitt ÌýDJÌýDevelopment of a gas chromatography-mass spectrometry method for determination of ketamine in plasma and its application to human samples.ÌýÌýTher Drug Monit. 1995;1795-Ìý100
53.
Woerner ÌýMGManuzza ÌýSKane ÌýJMÌýAnchoring the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale: an aid to improve reliability.ÌýÌýPsychopharmacol Bull. 1988;24112-Ìý117
54.
Teng ÌýELChui ÌýHCÌýThe modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) Examination.ÌýÌýJ Clin Psychiatry. 1987;48314-Ìý318
55.
Hirayasu ÌýYPotts ÌýGFO'Donnell ÌýBFKwon ÌýJSArakaki ÌýHAkdag ÌýSJLevitt ÌýJJShenton ÌýMEMcCarley ÌýRWÌýAuditory mismatch negativity in schizophrenia: topographic evaluation with a high-density recording montage.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1998;1551281-Ìý1284Google Scholar
56.
Barch ÌýDMCarter ÌýCSMacdonald ÌýASabb ÌýFWNoll ÌýDCCohen ÌýJDÌýPrefrontal cortex and context processing in medication-naive first-episode patients with schizophrenia.ÌýÌýSchizophr Res. 1999;36217-Ìý218
57.
Javitt ÌýDCSchroeder ÌýCESteinschneider ÌýMArezzo ÌýJCVaughan ÌýHG ÌýJrÌýDemonstration of mismatch negativity in the monkey.ÌýÌýElectroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1992;8387-Ìý90
58.
Sherman ÌýADHegwood ÌýTSBaruah ÌýSWaziri ÌýRÌýDeficient NMDA-mediated glutamate release from synaptosomes of schizophrenics.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1991;301191-Ìý1198
59.
Akbarian ÌýSSucher ÌýNJBradley ÌýDTafazzoli ÌýATrinh ÌýDHetrick ÌýWPPotkin ÌýSGSandman ÌýCABunney ÌýWE ÌýJrJones ÌýEGÌýSelective alterations in gene expression for NMDA receptor subunits in prefrontal cortex of schizophrenics.ÌýÌýJ Neurosci. 1996;1619-Ìý30
60.
Sokolov ÌýBPÌýExpression of NMDAR1, GluR1, GluR7, and KA1 glutamate receptor mRNAs is decreased in frontal cortex of "neuroleptic-free" schizophrenics: evidence on reversible up-regulation by typical neuroleptics.ÌýÌýJ Neurochem. 1998;712454-Ìý2464
61.
Mohn ÌýARGainetdinov ÌýRRCaron ÌýMGKoller ÌýBHÌýMice with reduced NMDA receptor expression display behaviors related to schizophrenia.ÌýÌý°ä±ð±ô±ô. 1999;98427-Ìý436
62.
Humphries ÌýCMortimer ÌýAHirsch ÌýSde Belleroche ÌýJÌýNMDA receptor mRNA correlation with antemortem cognitive impairment in schizophrenia.ÌýÌý±·±ð³Ü°ù´Ç°ù±ð±è´Ç°ù³Ù. 1996;72051-Ìý2055
63.
Lim ÌýKOHedehus ÌýMMoseley ÌýMde Crespigny ÌýASullivan ÌýEVPfefferbaum ÌýAÌýCompromised white matter tract integrity in schizophrenia inferred from diffusion tensor imaging.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56367-Ìý374
64.
Lim ÌýKOAdalsteinsson ÌýESpielman ÌýDSullivan ÌýEVRosenbloom ÌýMJPfefferbaum ÌýAÌýProton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging of cortical gray and white matter in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55346-Ìý352
65.
Näätänen ÌýRÌýAttention and Brain Function.Ìý Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence A Erlbaum Associates1992;
66.
Oranje ÌýBvan Berckel ÌýBNMKemner ÌýCvan Ree ÌýJMKahn ÌýRSVerbaten ÌýMNÌýThe effects of a subanaethetic dose of ketamine on human selective attention.ÌýÌý±·±ð³Ü°ù´Ç±è²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²¹°ù³¾²¹³¦´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 2000;22293-Ìý302
67.
Alho ÌýKÌýCerebral generators of mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic counterpart (MMNm) elicited by sound changes.ÌýÌýEar Hear. 1995;1638-Ìý51
68.
Kropotov ÌýJDNäätänen ÌýRSevostianov ÌýAVAlho ÌýKReinikainen ÌýKKropotova ÌýOVÌýMismatch negativity to auditory stimulus change recorded directly from the human temporal cortex.ÌýÌý±Ê²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²â²õ¾±´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1995;32418-Ìý422
69.
Alain ÌýCWoods ÌýDLKnight ÌýRTÌýA distributed cortical network for auditory sensory memory in humans.ÌýÌýBrain Res. 1998;81223-Ìý37
70.
Alho ÌýKWoods ÌýDLAlgazi ÌýAKnight ÌýRTNäätänen ÌýRÌýLesions of frontal cortex diminish the auditory mismatch negativity.ÌýÌýElectroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1994;91353-Ìý362
71.
Born ÌýJBräuninger ÌýWFehm-Wolfsdorf ÌýGVoigt ÌýKPauschinger ÌýPFehm ÌýHLÌýDose-dependent influences on electrophysiological signs of attention in humans after neuropeptide ACTH 4-10.ÌýÌýExp Brain Res. 1987;6785-Ìý92
72.
Born ÌýJKern ÌýWPietrowsky ÌýRSittig ÌýWFehm ÌýHLÌýFragments of ACTH affect electrophysiological signs of controlled stimulus processing in humans.ÌýÌý±Ê²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²¹°ù³¾²¹³¦´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1989;99439-Ìý444
73.
Winsberg ÌýBGJavitt ÌýDCSilipo ÌýGSÌýElectrophysiological indices of information processing in methylphenidate responders.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1997;42434-Ìý445
74.
Jevtovic-Todorovic ÌýVTodorovic ÌýSMMennerick ÌýSPowell ÌýSDikranian ÌýKBenshoff ÌýNZorumski ÌýCFOlney ÌýJWÌýNitrous oxide (laughing gas) is an NMDA antagonist, neuroprotectant and neurotoxin.ÌýÌýNat Med. 1998;4460-Ìý463
75.
Mennerick ÌýSJevtovic-Todorovic ÌýVTodorovic ÌýSMShen ÌýWOlney ÌýJWZorumski ÌýCFÌýEffect of nitrous oxide on excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission in hippocampal cultures.ÌýÌýJ Neurosci. 1998;189716-Ìý9726
76.
Tsai ÌýGGastfriend ÌýDRCoyle ÌýJTÌýThe glutamatergic basis of human alcoholism.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1995;152332-Ìý340
77.
Jääskeläinen ÌýIPLehtokoski ÌýAAlho ÌýKKujala ÌýTPekkonen ÌýESinclair ÌýJDNäätänen ÌýRSillanaukee ÌýPÌýLow dose of ethanol suppresses mismatch negativity of auditory event-related potentials.ÌýÌýAlcohol Clin Exp Res. 1995;19607-Ìý610
78.
Jääskeläinen ÌýIPPekkonen ÌýEAlho ÌýKSinclair ÌýJDSillanaukee ÌýPNäätänen ÌýRÌýDose-related effect of alcohol on mismatch negativity and reaction time performance.ÌýÌý´¡±ô³¦´Ç³ó´Ç±ô. 1995;12491-Ìý495
79.
Jääskeläinen ÌýIPPekkonen ÌýEHirvonen ÌýJSillanaukee ÌýPNäätänen ÌýRÌýMismatch negativity subcomponents and ethyl alcohol.ÌýÌýBiol Psychol. 1996;4313-Ìý25
80.
Pang ÌýEWFowler ÌýBÌýDissociation of the mismatch negativity and processing negativity attentional waveforms with nitrous oxide.ÌýÌý±Ê²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²â²õ¾±´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1999;36552-Ìý558
81.
Oye ÌýIHustveit ÌýOMaurset ÌýARatti Moberg ÌýEPaulsen ÌýOSkoglund ÌýLAÌýThe chiral forms of ketamine as probes for NMDA-receptor function in humans.ÌýKameyama ÌýTNabeshima ÌýTDomino ÌýEFeds.ÌýNMDA Receptor Related Agents Biochemistry, Pharmacology and Behavior Ann Arbor, Mich NPP Books1991;381-Ìý389
82.
Hustveit ÌýOMaurset ÌýAOye ÌýIÌýInteraction of the chiral forms of ketamine with opioid, phencyclidine, sigma and muscarinic receptors.ÌýÌýPharmacol Toxicol. 1995;77355-Ìý359
83.
Moghaddam ÌýBAdams ÌýBVerma ÌýADaly ÌýDÌýActivation of glutamatergic neurotransmission by ketamine: a novel step in the pathway from NMDA receptor blockade to dopaminergic and cognitive disruptions associated with the prefrontal cortex.ÌýÌýJ Neurosci. 1997;172921-Ìý2927
84.
Moghaddam ÌýBAdams ÌýBWÌýReversal of phencyclidine effects by a group II metabotropic glutamate receptor agonist in rats.ÌýÌý³§³¦¾±±ð²Ô³¦±ð. 1998;2811349-Ìý1352
85.
Aghajanian ÌýGKMarek ÌýGJÌýSerotonin, via 5-HT2A receptors, increases EPSCs in layer V pyramidal cells of prefrontal cortex by an asynchronous mode of glutamate release.ÌýÌýBrain Res. 1999;825161-Ìý171
86.
Umbricht ÌýDVollenweider ÌýFXSchmid ÌýLKoller ÌýRÌýInvestigations into the neurobiology of information processing deficits in schizophrenia.ÌýÌýSchizophr Res. 2000;41290-Ìý291
87.
Umbricht ÌýDVollenweider ÌýFXÌýEffects of NMDA-antagonists and 5-HT2A-agonists on generation of MMN in human volunteers [abstract].ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1999;4551³§-Ìý52³§
×