vlog

Object moved to here.

Subjective Cognitive Decline Plus and Longitudinal Assessment and Risk for Cognitive Impairment | Genetics and Genomics | JAMA Psychiatry | vlog

vlog

[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. Description and Exemplar Participants Demonstrating How Survival Time Was Calculated for Those in the Subjective Cognitive Decline (SCD) and Non-SCD Groups

A visit was considered valid if the participant was cognitively normal at the time of the visit and the question about SCD was administered. SCD was treated as time-varying; therefore, some participants may have contributed time to both the SCD and non-SCD groups. Participants were considered to have SCD beginning at their baseline visit and for all time subsequently if they met 1 of the following criteria: (1) SCD was endorsed at the baseline visit and all subsequent valid visits or (2) participants had only 1 valid visit during which SCD was endorsed. For participants who changed their responses across valid visits, SCD was considered present if endorsed at the last valid visit. If SCD was endorsed during consecutive valid visits, including the last, the participant was considered to have SCD beginning at the first of these consecutive visits—prior visits counted toward time without SCD. Participants were not considered to have SCD at the baseline visit and for all time subsequently if SCD was not endorsed at the last valid visit. We defined 4 time periods to calculate the survival time for SCD and non-SCD groups: (1) SCD time in red was the time from the first to the last consecutive valid visit during which SCD was endorsed, provided it was endorsed at the last valid visit, (2) cognitively normal time in blue was the time from the first to the last valid visit, (3) occurring time in orange was the time from the last valid visit to the onset of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or dementia (depending on which analysis), and (4) contacted time in green was the time from the last valid visit to the last contact or death for participants who did not develop MCI or dementia. For participants who contributed time to the SCD group (regardless of whether they also contributed time to the non-SCD group), the survival time was calculated in the following ways: (1) the sum of SCD time and occurring time for those who developed MCI or dementia (eg, participants A and D), (2) the sum of SCD time and contacted time for those who did not develop MCI or dementia (eg, participant B). For participants who only contributed time to the non-SCD group, the survival time was calculated in the following ways: (1) the sum of cognitively normal time and occurring time for those who developed MCI or dementia (eg, participant C), (2) the sum of cognitively normal time and contacted time for those who did not develop MCI or dementia (eg, participant E). For participants who contributed time to both the non-SCD and SCD groups, time contributed to the non-SCD group was calculated as the difference between cognitively normal time and SCD time (eg, participants B and D).

Figure 2. Flowchart of the Study Sample Selection

A, Among 3735 participants from the hybrid set of visits, 1651 and 2663 participants were classified into the subjective cognitive decline (SCD) and non-SCD groups, respectively—579 participants (15.5%) who changed their SCD status contributed time to both groups. A total of 3585 participants (96.0%) had available information for the APOE genotypes, and among those with APOE genotypes, 3057 (81.8%) had available data to derive an Alzheimer disease (AD) polygenic risk score (PRS). B, Among 2692 participants from the set of neuropsychological (NP) visits, 1194 and 1740 participants were classified into the SCD and non-SCD groups, respectively—242 participants (9.0%) who answered inconsistently contributed time to both groups. A total of 2581 participants (95.9%) had available information for the APOE genotypes, and among those with APOE genotypes, 2207 (82.0%) had available data to derive an AD PRS.

Figure 3. Kaplan-Meier Curves for Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI), Alzheimer Disease (AD), and All-Cause Dementia in Survival Models With APOE Adjustment

We used Cox proportional hazards models adjusted for age, sex, educational level, APOE ε4 status, and APOE ε2 status to plot the Kaplan-Meier curve for MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia in the participants from the hybrid set of visits. Compared with the non-SCD group, the SCD group had significantly higher hazards of conversion to MCI, AD, or all-cause dementia.

Table 1. Characteristics of the Study Sample
Table 2. Survival Models With APOE Adjustment
1.
Schmand B, Jonker C, Hooijer C, Lindeboom J. Subjective memory complaints may announce dementia. ܰDZDz. 1996;46(1):121-125. doi:
2.
Clarnette RM, Almeida OP, Forstl H, Paton A, Martins RN. Clinical characteristics of individuals with subjective memory loss in Western Australia: results from a cross-sectional survey. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2001;16(2):168-174. doi:
3.
Reisberg B, Shulman MB, Torossian C, Leng L, Zhu W. Outcome over 7 years of healthy adults with and without subjective cognitive impairment. Alzheimers Dement. 2010;6(1):11-24. doi:
4.
Hong YJ, Lee JH. Subjective cognitive decline and Alzheimer disease spectrum disorder. Dement Neurocogn Disord. 2017;16(2):40-47. doi:
5.
Saykin AJ, Wishart HA, Rabin LA, et al. Older adults with cognitive complaints show brain atrophy similar to that of amnestic MCI. ܰDZDz. 2006;67(5):834-842. doi:
6.
Barnes LL, Schneider JA, Boyle PA, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Memory complaints are related to Alzheimer disease pathology in older persons. ܰDZDz. 2006;67(9):1581-1585. doi:
7.
Mosconi L, De Santi S, Brys M, et al. Hypometabolism and altered cerebrospinal fluid markers in normal apolipoprotein E ε4 carriers with subjective memory complaints. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(6):609-618. doi:
8.
Liew TM. Subjective cognitive decline, APOE e4 allele, and the risk of neurocognitive disorders: age- and sex-stratified cohort study. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2022;56(12):1664-1675. doi:
9.
Petersen RC, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, et al. NIA-AA Alzheimer’s Disease Framework: clinical characterization of stages. Ann Neurol. 2021;89(6):1145-1156. doi:
10.
Jack CR Jr, Bennett DA, Blennow K, et al; Contributors. NIA-AA Research Framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(4):535-562. doi:
11.
Farias ST, Mungas D, Reed BR, Harvey D, DeCarli C. Progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia in clinic- vs community-based cohorts. Arch Neurol. 2009;66(9):1151-1157. doi:
12.
Tsuang D, Kukull W, Sheppard L, et al. Impact of sample selection on APOE epsilon 4 allele frequency: a comparison of 2 Alzheimer disease samples. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1996;44(6):704-707. doi:
13.
Jonker C, Geerlings MI, Schmand B. Are memory complaints predictive for dementia—a review of clinical and population-based studies. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;15(11):983-991. doi:
14.
Rodríguez-Gómez O, Abdelnour C, Jessen F, Valero S, Boada M. Influence of sampling and recruitment methods in studies of subjective cognitive decline. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;48(suppl 1):S99-S107. doi:
15.
Snitz BE, Wang T, Cloonan YK, et al. Risk of progression from subjective cognitive decline to mild cognitive impairment: the role of study setting. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(6):734-742. doi:
16.
Geerlings MI, Jonker C, Bouter LM, Adèr HJ, Schmand B. Association between memory complaints and incident Alzheimer disease in elderly people with normal baseline cognition. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(4):531-537. doi:
17.
Riedel-Heller SG, Matschinger H, Schork A, Angermeyer MC. Do memory complaints indicate the presence of cognitive impairment—results of a field study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;249(4):197-204. doi:
18.
Jorm AF, Masaki KH, Davis DG, et al. Memory complaints in nondemented men predict future pathologic diagnosis of Alzheimer disease. ܰDZDz. 2004;63(10):1960-1961. doi:
19.
van Oijen M, de Jong FJ, Hofman A, Koudstaal PJ, Breteler MM. Subjective memory complaints, education, and risk of Alzheimer disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2007;3(2):92-97. doi:
20.
Luck T, Luppa M, Briel S, et al. Mild cognitive impairment: incidence and risk factors: results of the Leipzig Longitudinal Study of the Aged. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010;58(10):1903-1910. doi:
21.
Pérès K, Helmer C, Amieva H, et al. Gender differences in the prodromal signs of dementia: memory complaint and IADL-restriction. a prospective population-based cohort. J Alzheimers Dis. 2011;27(1):39-47. doi:
22.
Chary E, Amieva H, Pérès K, Orgogozo JM, Dartigues JF, Jacqmin-Gadda H. Short- vs long-term prediction of dementia among subjects with low and high educational levels. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9(5):562-571. doi:
23.
Liew TM. Depression, subjective cognitive decline, and the risk of neurocognitive disorders. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2019;11(1):70. doi:
24.
Gifford KA, Liu D, Lu Z, et al. The source of cognitive complaints predicts diagnostic conversion differentially among nondemented older adults. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(3):319-327. doi:
25.
Rönnlund M, Sundström A, Adolfsson R, Nilsson LG. Subjective memory impairment in older adults predicts future dementia independent of baseline memory performance: evidence from the Betula prospective cohort study. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11(11):1385-1392. doi:
26.
Heser K, Kleineidam L, Wiese B, et al. Subjective cognitive decline may be a stronger predictor of incident dementia in women than in men. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;68(4):1469-1478. doi:
27.
Slot RER, Sikkes SAM, Berkhof J, et al; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative; DESCRIPA working group; INSIGHT-preAD study group; SCD-I working group. Subjective cognitive decline and rates of incident Alzheimer disease and non–Alzheimer disease dementia. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;15(3):465-476. doi:
28.
Liew TM. Trajectories of subjective cognitive decline, and the risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2020;12(1):135. doi:
29.
Molinuevo JL, Rabin LA, Amariglio R, et al; Subjective Cognitive Decline Initiative (SCD-I) Working Group. Implementation of subjective cognitive decline criteria in research studies. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13(3):296-311. doi:
30.
Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel D, et al. Association of apolipoprotein E allele epsilon 4 with late-onset familial and sporadic Alzheimer disease. ܰDZDz. 1993;43(8):1467-1472. doi:
31.
Nalbantoglu J, Gilfix BM, Bertrand P, et al. Predictive value of apolipoprotein E genotyping in Alzheimer disease: results of an autopsy series and an analysis of several combined studies. Ann Neurol. 1994;36(6):889-895. doi:
32.
Farrer LA, Cupples LA, Haines JL, et al. Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease: a meta-analysis—APOE and Alzheimer Disease Meta-Analysis Consortium. Ѵ. 1997;278(16):1349-1356. doi:
33.
Jessen F, Wolfsgruber S, Wiese B, et al; German Study on Aging, Cognition and Dementia in Primary Care Patients. AD dementia risk in late MCI, in early MCI, and in subjective memory impairment. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(1):76-83. doi:
34.
Hong YJ, Yoon B, Shim YS, et al. Predictors of clinical progression of subjective memory impairment in elderly subjects: data from the clinical research centers for dementia of South Korea (CREDOS). Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2015;40(3-4):158-165. doi:
35.
Ali JI, Smart CM, Gawryluk JR. Subjective cognitive decline and APOE ε4: a systematic review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;65(1):303-320. doi:
36.
Müller-Gerards D, Weimar C, Abramowski J, et al; Heinz Nixdorf Recall Study Investigative Group. Subjective cognitive decline, APOE ε4, and incident mild cognitive impairment in men and women. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2019;11(1):221-230. doi:
37.
Richardson TG, Harrison S, Hemani G, Davey Smith G. An atlas of polygenic risk score associations to highlight putative causal relationships across the human phenome. ڱ. 2019;8:e43657.doi:
38.
Jessen F, Amariglio RE, Buckley RF, et al. The characterization of subjective cognitive decline. Lancet Neurol. 2020;19(3):271-278. doi:
39.
Jessen F, Amariglio RE, van Boxtel M, et al; Subjective Cognitive Decline Initiative (SCD-I) Working Group. A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(6):844-852. doi:
40.
Dawber TR, Meadors GF, Moore FE Jr. Epidemiological approaches to heart disease: the Framingham Study. Am J Public Health Nations Health. 1951;41(3):279-281. doi:
41.
Feinleib M, Kannel WB, Garrison RJ, McNamara PM, Castelli WP. The Framingham Offspring Study—design and preliminary data. Prev Med. 1975;4(4):518-525. doi:
42.
Kannel WB, Feinleib M, McNamara PM, Garrison RJ, Castelli WP. An investigation of coronary heart disease in families: the Framingham Offspring Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1979;110(3):281-290. doi:
43.
Splansky GL, Corey D, Yang Q, et al. The third-generation cohort of the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute’s Framingham Heart Study: design, recruitment, and initial examination. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;165(11):1328-1335. doi:
44.
Farmer ME, White LR, Kittner SJ, et al. Neuropsychological test performance in Framingham: a descriptive study. Psychol Rep. 1987;60(3 Pt 2):1023-1040. doi:
45.
Au R, Seshadri S, Wolf PA, et al. New norms for a new generation: cognitive performance in the Framingham Offspring Cohort. Exp Aging Res. 2004;30(4):333-358. doi:
46.
Gmitrowicz A, Kucharska A. Developmental disorders in the fourth edition of the American classification: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual Of Mental Disorders (DSM IV–optional book). Article in Polish. Psychiatr Pol. 1994;28(5):509-521.
47.
McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of department of health and human services task force on Alzheimer disease. ܰDZDz. 1984;34(7):939-944. doi:
48.
Bachman DL, Wolf PA, Linn R, et al. Prevalence of dementia and probable senile dementia of the Alzheimer type in the Framingham Study. ܰDZDz. 1992;42(1):115-119. doi:
49.
Au R, Seshadri S, Knox K, et al. The Framingham Brain Donation Program: neuropathology along the cognitive continuum. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2012;9(6):673-686. doi:
50.
Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56(3):303-308. doi:
51.
Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, et al. Mild cognitive impairment—beyond controversies, toward a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):240-246. doi:
52.
Hixson JE, Vernier DT. Restriction isotyping of human apolipoprotein E by gene amplification and cleavage with HhaI. J Lipid Res. 1990;31(3):545-548. doi:
53.
Zhong L, Xie YZ, Cao TT, et al. A rapid and cost-effective method for genotyping apolipoprotein E gene polymorphism. Mol Neurodegener. 2016;11(1):2. doi:
54.
Yip AG, McKee AC, Green RC, et al. APOE, vascular pathology, and the AD brain. ܰDZDz. 2005;65(2):259-265. doi:
55.
Kunkle BW, Grenier-Boley B, Sims R, et al; Alzheimer Disease Genetics Consortium (ADGC); European Alzheimer’s Disease Initiative (EADI); Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology Consortium (CHARGE); Genetic and Environmental Risk in AD/Defining Genetic, Polygenic and Environmental Risk for Alzheimer’s Disease Consortium (GERAD/PERADES). Genetic meta-analysis of diagnosed Alzheimer disease identifies new risk loci and implicates Aβ, tau, immunity, and lipid processing. Nat Genet. 2019;51(3):414-430. doi:
56.
Ge T, Chen CY, Ni Y, Feng YA, Smoller JW. Polygenic prediction via Bayesian regression and continuous shrinkage priors. Nat Commun. 2019;10(1):1776. doi:
57.
Scollard P, Choi SE, Lee ML, et al. Ceiling effects and differential measurement precision across calibrated cognitive scores in the Framingham Study. ܰDZ⳦DZDz. 2023;37(4):383-397. doi:
58.
Nicholas CR, Dowling NM, Racine AM, et al. Longitudinal assessment of self- and informant-subjective cognitive complaints in a sample of healthy late-middle aged adults enriched with a family history of Alzheimer disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2017;23(8):617-626. doi:
59.
Pike KE, Cavuoto MG, Li L, Wright BJ, Kinsella GJ. Subjective cognitive decline: Level of risk for future dementia and mild cognitive impairment, a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Neuropsychol Rev. 2022;32(4):703-735. doi:
60.
Jessen F, Wiese B, Bachmann C, et al; German Study on Aging, Cognition and Dementia in Primary Care Patients Study Group. Prediction of dementia by subjective memory impairment: effects of severity and temporal association with cognitive impairment. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(4):414-422. doi:
1 Comment for this article
EXPAND ALL
Comment on "Subjective Cognitive Decline Plus and Longitudinal Assessment and Risk for Cognitive Impairment"
Angelo Torrente, MD | Department of Biomedicine, Neuroscience and Advanced Diagnostics (Bi.N.D.), University of Palermo, 90129, Via La Loggia 1, Palermo, Italy
Subjective cognitive decline (SCD) is a clinical construct that has recently been identified as a risk factor for cognitive deterioration [1]. This condition is based on persistent, self-perceived cognitive disturbances without objective cognitive impairment. Certain characteristics increase the likelihood of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (i.e. the so-called SCD plus, SCD+)[1], including: subjective memory-related decline, onset of SCD within the last 5 years, age at onset ≥60 years, worries associated with SCD, feeling less performant than people of the same age, confirmation of cognitive decline by an informant, apolipoprotein (APOE) ε4 genotype and biomarkers of AD. We read with interest the article by Kang et al, which is the largest prospective study to date on the evolution of SCD towards Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and dementia [2]; in particular, SCD+ was significantly associated with an increased risk of incident MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia, regardless of the presence of APOE ε4.
We would like to discuss some methodological considerations regarding this article. First, it should be considered that there may be subjects who revert to non-SCD at follow-up (probably due to a behavioral trait) [3]. Unfortunately, Kang et al. in their study did not evaluate the different trajectories of persistent vs. non-persistent SCD in increasing the risk for MCI and dementia. On the other hand, it could be speculated that those who no longer complain of SCD may have lost awareness of the disease. Second, regarding comorbidities, residual confounding can be assumed. Indeed, although the authors assessed the presence of depression and cardiovascular disease, they did not consider other relevant disorders such as sleep disorders, which has been described as a robust predictor of SCD[3]. Last, SCD has also been shown to be negatively associated with health-related quality of life [4], interacting with cognitive reserve in increasing the risk of dementia at follow-up [5]. Therefore, also these factors should be considered in future prospective work on SCD.
In conclusion, SCD is a highly prevalent condition that deserves to be studied to early identify AD and dementia. However, future studies should also evaluate the possibility of prospective reversion to a non-SCD state with probable behavioral genesis, by multimodally assessing the presence of somatic comorbidities and including factors such as cognitive reserve and health-related quality of life. Such data will be critical to better assess and define the prognostic value of SCD. Roberto Monastero, MD, PhD, Associate Professor; Angelo Torrente, MD; Nicola Veronese, MD.

References
1. Jessen F, Amariglio RE, Van Boxtel M, Breteler M, Ceccaldi M, Chételat G, et al. A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s and Dementia. 2014 Nov 1;10(6):844–52.
2. Kang M, Li C, Mahajan A, Spat-Lemus J, Durape S, Chen J, et al. Subjective Cognitive Decline Plus and Longitudinal Assessment and Risk for Cognitive Impairment. JAMA Psychiatry [Internet]. 2024 Jul 3;
3. Ball HA, Coulthard E, Fish M, Bayer A, Gallacher J, Ben-Shlomo Y. Predictors and prognosis of population-based subjective cognitive decline: longitudinal evidence from the Caerphilly Prospective Study (CaPS). BMJ Open. 2023 Oct 16;13(10):e073205. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2023-073205. PMID: 37844990; PMCID: PMC10582873.
4. Königsberg A, Belau MH, Ascone L, Gallinat J, Kühn S, Jensen M, Gerloff C, Cheng B, Thomalla G. Subjective
CONFLICT OF INTEREST: None Reported
READ MORE
Original Investigation
July 3, 2024

Subjective Cognitive Decline Plus and Longitudinal Assessment and Risk for Cognitive Impairment

Author Affiliations
  • 1Framingham Heart Study, Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 2Department of Medicine (Biomedical Genetics), Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York
  • 4George Washington University School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Washington, DC
  • 5Department of Psychology, Montclair State University, Montclair, New Jersey
  • 6Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 7Department of Neurology, Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 8Department of Biostatistics, Boston University School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 9Department of Anatomy & Neurobiology, Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 10Slone Epidemiology Center, Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 11Department of Pharmacology & Experimental Therapeutics, Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 12Department of Psychiatry, Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 13Department of Epidemiology, Boston University School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 14Department of Ophthalmology, Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts
JAMA Psychiatry. 2024;81(10):993-1002. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2024.1678
Key Points

Question Among cognitively normal adults in the community, what is the risk from subjective cognitive decline (SCD), using SCD-plus (SCD+) criteria and assessed longitudinally, associated with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), Alzheimer disease (AD), and all-cause dementia?

Findings In this cohort study using longitudinal data from the Framingham Heart Study including 3585 participants, SCD+ was significantly associated with survival time to MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia. Associations were independent of APOE status and an AD polygenic risk score.

Meaning In a community setting, SCD+ was associated with an increased risk of future MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia with similar hazards estimated in clinic-based settings.

Abstract

Importance Subjective cognitive decline (SCD) is recognized to be in the Alzheimer disease (AD) cognitive continuum. The SCD Initiative International Working Group recently proposed SCD-plus (SCD+) features that increase risk for future objective cognitive decline but that have not been assessed in a large community-based setting.

Objective To assess SCD risk for mild cognitive impairment (MCI), AD, and all-cause dementia, using SCD+ criteria among cognitively normal adults.

Design, Setting, and Participants The Framingham Heart Study, a community-based prospective cohort study, assessed SCD between 2005 and 2019, with up to 12 years of follow-up. Participants 60 years and older with normal cognition at analytic baseline were included. Cox proportional hazards (CPH) models were adjusted for baseline age, sex, education, APOE ε4 status, and tertiles of AD polygenic risk score (PRS), excluding the APOE region. Data were analyzed from May 2021 to November 2023.

Exposure SCD was assessed longitudinally using a single question and considered present if endorsed at the last cognitively normal visit. It was treated as a time-varying variable, beginning at the first of consecutive, cognitively normal visits, including the last, at which it was endorsed.

Main Outcomes and Measures Consensus-diagnosed MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia.

Results This study included 3585 participants (mean [SD] baseline age, 68.0 [7.7] years; 1975 female [55.1%]). A total of 1596 participants (44.5%) had SCD, and 770 (21.5%) were carriers of APOE ε4. APOE ε4 and tertiles of AD PRS status did not significantly differ between the SCD and non-SCD groups. MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia were diagnosed in 236 participants (6.6%), 73 participants (2.0%), and 89 participants (2.5%), respectively, during follow-up. On average, SCD preceded MCI by 4.4 years, AD by 6.8 years, and all-cause dementia by 6.9 years. SCD was significantly associated with survival time to MCI (hazard ratio [HR], 1.57; 95% CI, 1.22-2.03; P <.001), AD (HR, 2.98; 95% CI, 1.89-4.70; P <.001), and all-cause dementia (HR, 2.14; 95% CI, 1.44-3.18; P <.001). After adjustment for APOE and AD PRS, the hazards of SCD were largely unchanged.

Conclusions and Relevance Results of this cohort study suggest that in a community setting, SCD reflecting SCD+ features was associated with an increased risk of future MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia with similar hazards estimated in clinic-based settings. SCD may be an independent risk factor for AD and other dementias beyond the risk incurred by APOE ε4 and AD PRS.

Introduction

Early detection of Alzheimer disease (AD) in its preclinical stages is critical for effective intervention and prevention. Subjective cognitive decline (SCD) has been recognized as a feature of preclinical AD and other dementias.1-3 SCD refers to a self-perception of cognitive decline despite normal performance on traditional neuropsychological testing.4 Numerous studies demonstrate that SCD may be an early marker for cognitive decline or the presence of AD pathology, even for those individuals with no objective cognitive deficits.3,5-8 The updated AD research framework of the National Institute on Aging and Alzheimer Association now recognizes SCD in the cognitive continuum within the cognitively unimpaired stage.4,9,10

Many studies on SCD have been conducted in clinical or volunteer convenience samples who are easier to recruit and have higher rates of concerns about cognition than community samples. Clinical samples tend to have higher rates of conversion from normal cognition to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and from MCI to dementia than community samples. This is likely due to clinical samples having higher risk profiles for conversion (eg, higher rates of APOE ε4, family history of dementia, educational level, and income).11-13 Additionally, individuals in clinical settings seek help for their cognitive difficulties, which may be distinct and/or more severe than difficulties endorsed in community studies. As SCD may, at times, be a feature of normal aging rather than an intermediary between normal cognition and MCI, SCD may pose a smaller risk for conversion to MCI in community than in clinical samples because the baseline risk of conversion is also smaller in community samples.14,15 With caveats related to study design, the evidence bears this out, with SCD often showing larger effects for conversion in clinic samples and smaller or absent effects in community samples.1,15-27 These differences are consequential because community studies are less susceptible to selection bias and are more representative of the general population where early screening would usually take place.

To date, community studies of SCD have been limited by 1 or more of the following: small sample size, a single SCD assessment, lack of careful cognitive assessment in all or most participants, or lack of a criterion-standard consensus diagnosis for AD and dementia.28,29 Assessing SCD over multiple time points with repeated expressions of concern could be crucial for improved SCD measurement. Careful cognitive assessment in all or most participants is also crucial to ensure that SCD is occurring in the absence of objective impairment. These improvements in study design could lead to a more accurate assessment of the true association between SCD and dementia in the general population.29

The APOE ε4 allele is a well-established genetic risk factor for developing AD.30-34 The relationships between APOE ε4, SCD, and objective impairment remain unclear,35 despite several studies on this topic. A systematic review35 found that among individuals with normal cognition, APOE ε4 frequency was comparable between those with and without SCD. As SCD is considered part of the cognitive continuum, the lack of association with APOE ε4 is surprising. Narrative reviews suggested that both APOE ε4 and SCD conferred an individual and multiplicative risk of conversion to objective cognitive impairment. This interaction between SCD and APOE ε4 status may be sex specific, with a significant association in women, but not men.8,36 Lastly, it seems plausible that genetic risk for AD beyond APOE ε4 may confer risk for SCD. A polygenic risk score (PRS) measures an individual’s genetic likelihood of having a particular trait over multiple genomic variants—in the case of AD PRS, it can demonstrate an individual’s propensity for developing AD.37 To our knowledge, the association between an AD PRS and SCD has not been evaluated.

Given these knowledge gaps, we investigated the association between SCD and risk of conversion to MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia, using data collected in the Framingham Heart Study (FHS), a large community study with comprehensively characterized cognition and criterion-standard consensus dementia diagnoses. We leveraged data collected on SCD across multiple visits at which participants were objectively cognitively normal and estimated the hazard ratio (HR) of conversion in the Cox proportional hazards (CPH) model. Based on recommendations from the international working group known as the SCD Initiative, we incorporated into the study design several characteristics of SCD (termed SCD-plus features, hereafter referred to as SCD+) suggested to increase risk for future objective cognitive decline.38,39 We aimed to (1) examine the association between SCD and conversion to MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia, (2) examine how the size of associations may change with introduction of SCD+ features, (3) compare HRs estimated with and without adjustment for AD genetic risk factors (APOE ε4, AD PRS), and (4) test whether the interactions of SCD with these genetic risk factors and with sex, education, and depression (factors known to be associated with both SCD and AD) are associated with conversion.

Methods
Participants

The FHS is a prospective community longitudinal study, including multigenerational cohorts with initial recruitment in Framingham, Massachusetts.40-43 Participants were initially invited to a regular clinic examination, ie, core visit; neuropsychological testing was offered to all participants as part of a large ancillary study.44,45 The institutional review board at Boston University approved the study protocol. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. This study followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology () reporting guidelines.

The current study included participants with normal cognition at age 60 years or older. Visits before age 60 years were not considered, and participants with MCI or dementia at or before age 60 years were excluded. Participants were followed up from 2005 to 2019 for up to 12 years. Participants self-identified with the following race and ethnicity groups: Asian, non-Hispanic Black, Hispanic, non-Hispanic White, and other, which included Indian, Native American, and Pacific Islander. Race and ethnicity are included because they are well established risk factors for dementia.

To be included, participants must have completed items assessing SCD during at least 1 study visit at which time they were cognitively normal. We included all core and neuropsychological visits at which SCD was assessed and arranged visits longitudinally. We termed the combined set of core and/or neuropsychological visits as the hybrid set. The baseline visit was considered the first visit at or after age 60 years, at which SCD was assessed. More information on the methods used in this study are available in the eMethods in Supplement 1.

SCD

SCD items were presented in slightly different ways at core and neuropsychological visits. At core visits, participants were queried “Do you feel your memory is becoming worse?” At neuropsychological visits, participants were queried “Do you have any concerns about your memory or thinking?”

MCI and Dementia Diagnosis

Diagnoses and dates of onset of MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia were assigned by a dementia review panel, including neurologists and neuropsychologists. The panel used criteria from the DSM-IV to diagnose dementia,46 and criteria from the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer Disease and Related Disorders Association were used to diagnose AD.47,48 MCI was determined based on evidence of subjective and objective decline in one or more cognitive domains,49 meeting widely accepted criteria for MCI.50,51

APOE and PRS

APOE genotypes were determined from blood samples.52,53 Individuals were coded as ε4 carriers (ε2/ε4, ε3/ε4, and ε4/ε4) and ε2 carriers (ε2/ε2, ε2/ε3, and ε2/ε4).54 We derived 2 different PRSs (with and without the APOE region) using summary statistics from the AD genome-wide association study by Kunkle and colleagues.55 PRS-CS, a Bayesian PRS method using continuous shrinkage priors,56 was applied. Risk scores were evenly divided into low-, moderate-, and high-risk groups, and the categorized tertile values were considered in further analyses.

Statistical Analyses

A visit was considered valid if the participant was cognitively normal at the time of the visit and the question about SCD was administered. To compare basic characteristics between participants with and without SCD, participants were grouped into 1 of 3 categories that were consistent with the CPH models described subsequently. Participants in the only-SCD group endorsed SCD at each valid visit. Participants in the no-SCD group did not endorse SCD at their last valid visit. Participants in the mixed-SCD group endorsed SCD at their last valid visit but not at all valid visits. Comparisons used Fisher exact test for categorical variables and analysis of variance for continuous variables.

We used CPH models to compare time with MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia between those with and without SCD, which was treated as time varying. Figure 1 describes and shows exemplar participants demonstrating the different ways of SCD assignment and measuring survival time in years. Base models were adjusted for age, sex, and educational level. Age was defined as the age at the baseline visit in the non-SCD group and as the age at which SCD was considered to begin in the SCD group. Subsequent CPH models were adjusted for potential confounders, including genetic factors (APOE ε4 status, APOE ε2 status, and categorized PRS), depression, cardiovascular disease (CVD), current smoking, hypertension, and type 2 diabetes. Models accounted for the relatedness among individuals by using a mixed-model fitting familial structure as a random intercept. We constructed Kaplan-Meier curves for MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia, stratified by SCD and non-SCD groups, based on CPH models with age, sex, educational level, APOE ε4 status, and APOE ε2 status adjustments.

Additional sensitivity analyses investigated the SCD+ criteria. We limited models to participants from the set of neuropsychological visits only and the set of core visits only, as the SCD questions differed by whether concern was present (neuropsychological only). We shifted participants from the SCD to non-SCD group if SCD was consecutively endorsed for more than 5 years to better capture SCD onset within 5 years of objective impairment. We shifted participants from the SCD to the non-SCD group if SCD was endorsed only once at their last valid visit to better capture persistence of SCD. We made all 3 changes noted previously in the same model including only using neuropsychological visit (to capture concern), restricting consecutively endorsed SCD to 5 years, and requiring 2 consecutive visits with SCD, including the last. Data were analyzed from May 2021 to November 2023 using R software, version 4.3 (R Project for Statistical Computing). All P values were 2-sided, and a P value <.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results
Sample Characteristics

Overall, this study included 3585 individuals (mean [SD] baseline age, 68.0 [7.7] years; 1975 female [55.1%]; 1610 male [44.9%]). Participants identified with the following races and ethnicities: 90 Asian (2.5%), 113 non-Hispanic Black (3.2%), 97 Hispanic (2.7%), 3283 non-Hispanic White (91.6%), and 2 other (0.1%). There were 1804 college graduates (50.3%), and 770 APOE ε4 carriers (21.5%). A total of 236 participants (6.6%) developed MCI, 73 (2.0%) developed AD, and 89 (2.5%) developed all-cause dementia over the study period (Figure 2). On average, SCD preceded MCI by 4.4 years, AD by 6.8 years, and all-cause dementia by 6.9 years. Table 157 shows study sample characteristics overall and stratified into 3 groups: SCD at each valid visit (only SCD, n = 1034), SCD present at the last valid visit but not all valid visits (mixed SCD, n = 562; for a total of 1596 participants [44.5%] with SCD), and SCD not present at the last valid visit (no SCD, n = 1989). The baseline age across groups was similar, and the mean (SD) age of SCD beginning was 69.8 [8.0] years. Compared with the no-SCD group, the only-SCD group had significantly more women (613 of 1034 [59.3%] vs 1043 of 1989 [52.4%]) and depression (128 of 1034 [21.0%] vs 114 of 1989 [9.9%]). The groups did not significantly differ by APOE status or by other factors—CVD, current smoking, hypertension, and type 2 diabetes. Compared with the no-SCD group, future cognitive impairment was also significantly higher in the only-SCD group: MCI (89 of 1034 [8.6%] vs 115 of 1989 [5.8%]), AD (35 of 1034 [3.4%] vs 29 of 1989 [1.5%]), and all-cause dementia (40 of 1034 [3.9%] vs 39 of 1989 [2.0%]). Participants were followed up for 10 783 person-years and had a mean (SD) of 2.1 (1.5) valid visits. We observed the same trends in the set of neuropsychological visits (eTable 1 in Supplement 1).

Survival Analyses

Survival analyses revealed that SCD was significantly associated with survival time to MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia (Table 2 and Figure 3). In the base model (adjusting for age, sex, and educational level but not APOE status), the HR was 1.60 (95% CI, 1.24-2.06; P < .001) for MCI, 4.33 (95% CI, 2.38-7.85; P < .001) for AD, and 2.17 (95% CI, 1.46-3.23; P < .001) for all-cause dementia. When we included APOE ε4 status as a covariate, the HR of SCD remained similar for MCI (HR, 1.57; 95% CI, 1.22-2.03; P < .001) and all-cause dementia (HR, 2.14; 95% CI, 1.44-3.18; P < .001) but was reduced, although still significant, for AD (HR, 2.98; 95% CI, 1.89-4.70; P < .001). We also observed that the HR of APOE ε4 status was significant for MCI (HR, 1.72; 95% CI, 1.27-2.32; P < .001) and AD (HR, 1.88; 95% CI, 1.09-3.26; P = .02). In the model with both APOE ε4 and ε2 status, we found no significant association due to ε2 status, and the SCD HR remained similar.

When we included a categorized AD PRS as a covariate, we found no significant associations due to the PRS with and without the APOE region, and the SCD HR remained similar (eTables 2 and 3 in Supplement 1). In models including depression as a covariate, there were small reductions in the SCD HRs for all 3 outcomes (<5%), although the SCD hazards remained significant (eTable 3 in Supplement 1). When we included other CVD-related factors as covariates, the HR of SCD remained similar (eTable 3 in Supplement 1). We did not observe significant SCD interactions with sex, education, depression, or APOE ε4 for any outcomes (eTable 4 in Supplement 1). Three-way interactions among SCD, APOE ε4, and sex (or depression) were also not significant (eTable 4 in Supplement 1).

When we restricted participants to those from the set of neuropsychological visits (SCD with concern) (eTable 5 in Supplement 1), the HRs were modestly larger compared with models using the set of hybrid visits and with models using the set of core visits (eTable 6 in Supplement 1) for MCI and AD dementia but not for all-cause dementia. When we shifted participants from the SCD group to the non-SCD group if SCD was consecutively endorsed for more than 5 years, the APOE ε4adjusted HRs were increased by 30% to 50% compared with the primary models (eTable 7 in Supplement 1). When we shifted participants from the SCD group who endorsed SCD only once at their last valid visit (eTable 8 in Supplement 1), the APOE ε4adjusted HRs were not consistently different compared with the primary models. When we incorporated all 3 of these conditions, the APOE ε4adjusted HRs for MCI, AD dementia, and all-cause dementia were 7.62 (95% CI, 5.44-10.66), 6.02 (95% CI, 3.01-12.05), and 4.44 (95% CI, 2.40-8.20), respectively, values that are 1.6- to 4.5-fold larger than the main analyses (eTable 9 in Supplement 1).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this was the largest study of SCD in a community sample with a longitudinal assessment of SCD. We found strong associations of SCD with the risk of incident MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia, which are consistent with previous reports suggesting that SCD may be an early predictor of objective measures of cognitive impairment1-3,28,58 and AD pathology.5-7 On average, SCD preceded MCI by 4.4 years, AD by 6.8 years, and all-cause dementia by 6.9 years, supporting the premise that SCD could be an early manifestation of neurodegeneration, located on a continuum between normal cognition and MCI.5 Also, we showed that the hazard of SCD on future cognitive decline was independent of APOE ε4 status. We did not find differences by sex, education, APOE ε4 status, or an AD PRS in the association of SCD on our outcomes. Among potential confounding factors, we found that depression was significantly associated with both SCD and the onset of cognitive impairment and dementia, but adjustment for depression did not substantially alter hazards of SCD on time to outcomes.

The international working group, the SCD Initiative, has described several characteristics of SCD (SCD+ features) that may increase the risk for future objective cognitive decline.38 We incorporated several of these characteristics into our primary analyses (SCD in memory, irrespective of other domains of cognition, SCD onset over the age of 60 years, consistent SCD over time), and/or sensitivity analyses (worries associated with SCD, onset of SCD within the past 5 years, and more rigid definition of consistent SCD over time). Overall, these analyses provided validation for the SCD+ features. The primary model demonstrated HRs that were similar to those of a recent meta-analysis59 (that combined community and clinic samples). Sensitivity analyses demonstrated HRs that were larger for worries associated with SCD and onset of SCD within the past 5 years. A sensitivity analysis that incorporated all the previously listed features demonstrated markedly larger HRs (1.6- to 4.5-fold larger HRs). Our study findings suggest that these recommendations were effective for identifying those who are more likely to convert and may be used for designing effective screening tools.

A recent meta-analysis59 of studies evaluating the association between SCD and time to MCI and dementia found the HRs for dementia and MCI to be 1.90 (95% CI, 1.52-2.36) and 1.72 (95% CI, 1.18-2.52), respectively. This meta-analysis combined studies conducted in clinic, volunteer, and community cohorts.59 Heterogeneity would be expected across these studies based on differences in recruitment sources. SCD may pose a smaller risk for conversion to MCI and dementia in community samples than in clinical samples because the baseline risk of conversion is smaller in community samples.14 In the meta-analysis,59 moderator analyses on recruitment sources demonstrated that SCD posed significantly greater risk of developing MCI in clinical cohorts compared with community cohorts. Although there were not statistically significant differences for similar studies of dementia, results were in the same direction, but there was lack of power to detect a significant difference. That the association sizes from our community study were similar to (and actually larger in sensitivity analyses than) the meta-analysis that combined studies of varying recruitment sources speaks to the value of the SCD+ criteria to improve prediction.

In survival models adjusted for genetic risk factors of AD, SCD remained significantly associated with our outcomes. Specifically, compared with models that were not adjusted for APOE ε4 status, in adjusted models, the magnitude of SCD HR was slightly reduced but still significant for MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia. Further, we did not find that SCD was significantly more common among APOE ε4 carriers than noncarriers. The heritability of AD is 56% to 79%, and approximately one-half of heritability is explained by APOE ε4.30-32 Interestingly, our findings suggest that the effects of SCD and APOE ε4 are largely independent and that SCD does not mediate the effect of APOE ε4 on the incidence of AD. Further, we observed no SCD × APOE ε4 or related 3-way interactions associated with time to MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia. Our findings are largely consistent with a meta-analysis indicating that APOE ε4 does not predispose individuals to developing SCD.35 It provides further evidence that the ʰε4 allele confers a similar risk for cognitive impairment regardless of whether SCD is present.8 Additionally, even after the APOE ε4 adjustment, the hazards of SCD were substantially larger than the hazards of APOE ε4 itself.

Given that SCD is thought to be an intermediary between normal cognition and MCI, but did not mediate the relationship between APOE ε4 and cognitive impairment, it seemed plausible that it may instead mediate the relationship between other (non–APOE ε4) AD genetic risk factors and AD. For this reason, we also tested whether the AD PRS group with the APOE region excluded differed by SCD status and tested whether adding it to the CPH models would impact the hazard of SCD on time to cognitive impairment. We found that the PRS groups did not differ by SCD status, and their inclusion in the CPH models had no significant impact on the association of SCD on time to cognitive impairment. These findings should be interpreted with caution because, unlike APOE ε4, the AD PRS was not associated with time to any of our outcomes, probably due to the relatively small number of AD cases in the study sample.

Of the 1596 participants with SCD, only 121 progressed to cognitive impairment during the study period, and accordingly, the sensitivity (51%) and specificity (56%) of SCD were fair. This is not unique to the FHS and has been observed elsewhere.60 Importantly, clinicians make judgments based on positive and negative predictive values of a test, not their sensitivity and specificity. By contrast, the negative predictive value of SCD was excellent (94%). For a patient who does not meet criteria for SCD, the probability of developing cognitive impairment in the future is small, and additional screening may not be warranted. For a population with a similar rate of dementia as that of the FHS, this may reduce the need for further screening of greater than 50% of the population.

Limitations

This study has limitations. The rates of MCI (6.6%), AD (2.0%), and all-cause dementia (2.5%) were low. Because FHS participants have access to more health care opportunities, their burden of chronic diseases tends to be lower. The SCD questions were not administered at all visits, which could have led to missed cases or altered identification of when SCD occurred. More comprehensive scales have been developed that measure SCD with more granularity. We did not use these scales to minimize burden on participants who undergo extensive assessment beyond SCD. Two additional SCD+ features (seeking medical help, observer confirmation of cognitive decline) have been proposed, but unfortunately, they were not systematically collected in our study. Both are more readily available in clinical samples. AD biomarkers were not available to help elucidate underlying pathology, and only a small subset came to autopsy. We did conduct separate analyses for AD dementia and all-cause dementia. Although an AD clinical syndrome (ie, amnestic multidomain dementia) may be explained by AD pathology, other pathologies are commonly comorbid and may contribute to an AD clinical syndrome and all-cause dementia. Plausibly, the anatomic brain region where pathology accumulates, more than the type of pathology, may influence SCD. Lastly, the study sample was composed primarily of White, non-Hispanic participants, half of whom were college graduates, and most of whom resided in the northeast US. Generalizability to other groups may be limited.

Conclusions

In this cohort study including a large community sample with a longitudinal assessment of SCD and an analytic approach that embraced SCD+ features, we found significant associations between SCD and time to MCI, AD, and all-cause dementia, with large magnitudes, similar to those observed in clinical settings. Our findings provide added evidence that even in the community, SCD, particularly with the SCD+ features, is an important harbinger of future objective cognitive decline and dementia and argues for its use for early screening in the community. Further, SCD appears to be an independent risk factor for AD, beyond the risk incurred by genetics.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: April 17, 2024.

Published Online: July 3, 2024. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2024.1678

Corresponding Author: Jesse Mez, MD, MS, Department of Neurology, Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine, 72 E Concord St, L-522 Instructional Bldg, Boston, MA 02118 (jessemez@bu.edu).

Author Contributions: Drs Mez and Kang had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Kang, Mahajan, Gurnani, Lunetta, Farrer, Mez.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Kang, Li, Spat-Lemus, Durape, Chen, Devine, Auerbach, Ang, Sherva, Qiu, Lunetta, Au, Farrer, Mez.

Drafting of the manuscript: Kang, Mahajan, Mez.

Critical review of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Kang, Li, Spat-Lemus, Durape, Chen, Gurnani, Devine, Auerbach, Ang, Sherva, Qiu, Lunetta, Au, Farrer, Mez.

Statistical analysis: Kang, Durape, Chen, Sherva, Mez.

Obtained funding: Au, Farrer, Mez.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Gurnani, Devine, Auerbach, Ang, Mez.

Supervision: Li, Spat-Lemus, Devine, Lunetta, Farrer, Mez.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Devine reported receiving grants from the National Institute on Aging during the conduct of the study. Dr Ang reported receiving grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) during the conduct of the study. Dr Au reported receiving grants from National Institute on Aging; scientific advisory board fees from Signant Health and Novo Nordisk; and consulting fees from Davos Alzheimer’s Collaborative nonprofit organization outside the submitted work. Dr Farrer reported receiving grants from the NIH during the conduct of the study. Dr Mez reported receiving grants from the NIH during the conduct of the study. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This work was supported by the Framingham Heart Study’s National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute contract (N01-HC-25195; HHSN268201500001I), and National Institutes of Health grants from the National Institute on Aging (R01AG008122, R01AG016495, R01AG033040, RF1AG054156, RF1AG062109, R01AG061028, U19AG068753, P30AG072978).

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Data Sharing Statement: See Supplement 2.

Additional Contributions: We thank the Framingham Heart Study (FHS) participants for their decades of dedication and the FHS staff for their hard work in collecting and preparing the data. No financial compensation was received for these contributions.

References
1.
Schmand B, Jonker C, Hooijer C, Lindeboom J. Subjective memory complaints may announce dementia. ܰDZDz. 1996;46(1):121-125. doi:
2.
Clarnette RM, Almeida OP, Forstl H, Paton A, Martins RN. Clinical characteristics of individuals with subjective memory loss in Western Australia: results from a cross-sectional survey. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2001;16(2):168-174. doi:
3.
Reisberg B, Shulman MB, Torossian C, Leng L, Zhu W. Outcome over 7 years of healthy adults with and without subjective cognitive impairment. Alzheimers Dement. 2010;6(1):11-24. doi:
4.
Hong YJ, Lee JH. Subjective cognitive decline and Alzheimer disease spectrum disorder. Dement Neurocogn Disord. 2017;16(2):40-47. doi:
5.
Saykin AJ, Wishart HA, Rabin LA, et al. Older adults with cognitive complaints show brain atrophy similar to that of amnestic MCI. ܰDZDz. 2006;67(5):834-842. doi:
6.
Barnes LL, Schneider JA, Boyle PA, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Memory complaints are related to Alzheimer disease pathology in older persons. ܰDZDz. 2006;67(9):1581-1585. doi:
7.
Mosconi L, De Santi S, Brys M, et al. Hypometabolism and altered cerebrospinal fluid markers in normal apolipoprotein E ε4 carriers with subjective memory complaints. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(6):609-618. doi:
8.
Liew TM. Subjective cognitive decline, APOE e4 allele, and the risk of neurocognitive disorders: age- and sex-stratified cohort study. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2022;56(12):1664-1675. doi:
9.
Petersen RC, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, et al. NIA-AA Alzheimer’s Disease Framework: clinical characterization of stages. Ann Neurol. 2021;89(6):1145-1156. doi:
10.
Jack CR Jr, Bennett DA, Blennow K, et al; Contributors. NIA-AA Research Framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(4):535-562. doi:
11.
Farias ST, Mungas D, Reed BR, Harvey D, DeCarli C. Progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia in clinic- vs community-based cohorts. Arch Neurol. 2009;66(9):1151-1157. doi:
12.
Tsuang D, Kukull W, Sheppard L, et al. Impact of sample selection on APOE epsilon 4 allele frequency: a comparison of 2 Alzheimer disease samples. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1996;44(6):704-707. doi:
13.
Jonker C, Geerlings MI, Schmand B. Are memory complaints predictive for dementia—a review of clinical and population-based studies. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;15(11):983-991. doi:
14.
Rodríguez-Gómez O, Abdelnour C, Jessen F, Valero S, Boada M. Influence of sampling and recruitment methods in studies of subjective cognitive decline. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;48(suppl 1):S99-S107. doi:
15.
Snitz BE, Wang T, Cloonan YK, et al. Risk of progression from subjective cognitive decline to mild cognitive impairment: the role of study setting. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(6):734-742. doi:
16.
Geerlings MI, Jonker C, Bouter LM, Adèr HJ, Schmand B. Association between memory complaints and incident Alzheimer disease in elderly people with normal baseline cognition. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(4):531-537. doi:
17.
Riedel-Heller SG, Matschinger H, Schork A, Angermeyer MC. Do memory complaints indicate the presence of cognitive impairment—results of a field study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;249(4):197-204. doi:
18.
Jorm AF, Masaki KH, Davis DG, et al. Memory complaints in nondemented men predict future pathologic diagnosis of Alzheimer disease. ܰDZDz. 2004;63(10):1960-1961. doi:
19.
van Oijen M, de Jong FJ, Hofman A, Koudstaal PJ, Breteler MM. Subjective memory complaints, education, and risk of Alzheimer disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2007;3(2):92-97. doi:
20.
Luck T, Luppa M, Briel S, et al. Mild cognitive impairment: incidence and risk factors: results of the Leipzig Longitudinal Study of the Aged. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010;58(10):1903-1910. doi:
21.
Pérès K, Helmer C, Amieva H, et al. Gender differences in the prodromal signs of dementia: memory complaint and IADL-restriction. a prospective population-based cohort. J Alzheimers Dis. 2011;27(1):39-47. doi:
22.
Chary E, Amieva H, Pérès K, Orgogozo JM, Dartigues JF, Jacqmin-Gadda H. Short- vs long-term prediction of dementia among subjects with low and high educational levels. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9(5):562-571. doi:
23.
Liew TM. Depression, subjective cognitive decline, and the risk of neurocognitive disorders. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2019;11(1):70. doi:
24.
Gifford KA, Liu D, Lu Z, et al. The source of cognitive complaints predicts diagnostic conversion differentially among nondemented older adults. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(3):319-327. doi:
25.
Rönnlund M, Sundström A, Adolfsson R, Nilsson LG. Subjective memory impairment in older adults predicts future dementia independent of baseline memory performance: evidence from the Betula prospective cohort study. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11(11):1385-1392. doi:
26.
Heser K, Kleineidam L, Wiese B, et al. Subjective cognitive decline may be a stronger predictor of incident dementia in women than in men. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;68(4):1469-1478. doi:
27.
Slot RER, Sikkes SAM, Berkhof J, et al; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative; DESCRIPA working group; INSIGHT-preAD study group; SCD-I working group. Subjective cognitive decline and rates of incident Alzheimer disease and non–Alzheimer disease dementia. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;15(3):465-476. doi:
28.
Liew TM. Trajectories of subjective cognitive decline, and the risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2020;12(1):135. doi:
29.
Molinuevo JL, Rabin LA, Amariglio R, et al; Subjective Cognitive Decline Initiative (SCD-I) Working Group. Implementation of subjective cognitive decline criteria in research studies. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13(3):296-311. doi:
30.
Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel D, et al. Association of apolipoprotein E allele epsilon 4 with late-onset familial and sporadic Alzheimer disease. ܰDZDz. 1993;43(8):1467-1472. doi:
31.
Nalbantoglu J, Gilfix BM, Bertrand P, et al. Predictive value of apolipoprotein E genotyping in Alzheimer disease: results of an autopsy series and an analysis of several combined studies. Ann Neurol. 1994;36(6):889-895. doi:
32.
Farrer LA, Cupples LA, Haines JL, et al. Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease: a meta-analysis—APOE and Alzheimer Disease Meta-Analysis Consortium. Ѵ. 1997;278(16):1349-1356. doi:
33.
Jessen F, Wolfsgruber S, Wiese B, et al; German Study on Aging, Cognition and Dementia in Primary Care Patients. AD dementia risk in late MCI, in early MCI, and in subjective memory impairment. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(1):76-83. doi:
34.
Hong YJ, Yoon B, Shim YS, et al. Predictors of clinical progression of subjective memory impairment in elderly subjects: data from the clinical research centers for dementia of South Korea (CREDOS). Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2015;40(3-4):158-165. doi:
35.
Ali JI, Smart CM, Gawryluk JR. Subjective cognitive decline and APOE ε4: a systematic review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;65(1):303-320. doi:
36.
Müller-Gerards D, Weimar C, Abramowski J, et al; Heinz Nixdorf Recall Study Investigative Group. Subjective cognitive decline, APOE ε4, and incident mild cognitive impairment in men and women. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2019;11(1):221-230. doi:
37.
Richardson TG, Harrison S, Hemani G, Davey Smith G. An atlas of polygenic risk score associations to highlight putative causal relationships across the human phenome. ڱ. 2019;8:e43657.doi:
38.
Jessen F, Amariglio RE, Buckley RF, et al. The characterization of subjective cognitive decline. Lancet Neurol. 2020;19(3):271-278. doi:
39.
Jessen F, Amariglio RE, van Boxtel M, et al; Subjective Cognitive Decline Initiative (SCD-I) Working Group. A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(6):844-852. doi:
40.
Dawber TR, Meadors GF, Moore FE Jr. Epidemiological approaches to heart disease: the Framingham Study. Am J Public Health Nations Health. 1951;41(3):279-281. doi:
41.
Feinleib M, Kannel WB, Garrison RJ, McNamara PM, Castelli WP. The Framingham Offspring Study—design and preliminary data. Prev Med. 1975;4(4):518-525. doi:
42.
Kannel WB, Feinleib M, McNamara PM, Garrison RJ, Castelli WP. An investigation of coronary heart disease in families: the Framingham Offspring Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1979;110(3):281-290. doi:
43.
Splansky GL, Corey D, Yang Q, et al. The third-generation cohort of the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute’s Framingham Heart Study: design, recruitment, and initial examination. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;165(11):1328-1335. doi:
44.
Farmer ME, White LR, Kittner SJ, et al. Neuropsychological test performance in Framingham: a descriptive study. Psychol Rep. 1987;60(3 Pt 2):1023-1040. doi:
45.
Au R, Seshadri S, Wolf PA, et al. New norms for a new generation: cognitive performance in the Framingham Offspring Cohort. Exp Aging Res. 2004;30(4):333-358. doi:
46.
Gmitrowicz A, Kucharska A. Developmental disorders in the fourth edition of the American classification: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual Of Mental Disorders (DSM IV–optional book). Article in Polish. Psychiatr Pol. 1994;28(5):509-521.
47.
McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of department of health and human services task force on Alzheimer disease. ܰDZDz. 1984;34(7):939-944. doi:
48.
Bachman DL, Wolf PA, Linn R, et al. Prevalence of dementia and probable senile dementia of the Alzheimer type in the Framingham Study. ܰDZDz. 1992;42(1):115-119. doi:
49.
Au R, Seshadri S, Knox K, et al. The Framingham Brain Donation Program: neuropathology along the cognitive continuum. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2012;9(6):673-686. doi:
50.
Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56(3):303-308. doi:
51.
Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, et al. Mild cognitive impairment—beyond controversies, toward a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):240-246. doi:
52.
Hixson JE, Vernier DT. Restriction isotyping of human apolipoprotein E by gene amplification and cleavage with HhaI. J Lipid Res. 1990;31(3):545-548. doi:
53.
Zhong L, Xie YZ, Cao TT, et al. A rapid and cost-effective method for genotyping apolipoprotein E gene polymorphism. Mol Neurodegener. 2016;11(1):2. doi:
54.
Yip AG, McKee AC, Green RC, et al. APOE, vascular pathology, and the AD brain. ܰDZDz. 2005;65(2):259-265. doi:
55.
Kunkle BW, Grenier-Boley B, Sims R, et al; Alzheimer Disease Genetics Consortium (ADGC); European Alzheimer’s Disease Initiative (EADI); Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology Consortium (CHARGE); Genetic and Environmental Risk in AD/Defining Genetic, Polygenic and Environmental Risk for Alzheimer’s Disease Consortium (GERAD/PERADES). Genetic meta-analysis of diagnosed Alzheimer disease identifies new risk loci and implicates Aβ, tau, immunity, and lipid processing. Nat Genet. 2019;51(3):414-430. doi:
56.
Ge T, Chen CY, Ni Y, Feng YA, Smoller JW. Polygenic prediction via Bayesian regression and continuous shrinkage priors. Nat Commun. 2019;10(1):1776. doi:
57.
Scollard P, Choi SE, Lee ML, et al. Ceiling effects and differential measurement precision across calibrated cognitive scores in the Framingham Study. ܰDZ⳦DZDz. 2023;37(4):383-397. doi:
58.
Nicholas CR, Dowling NM, Racine AM, et al. Longitudinal assessment of self- and informant-subjective cognitive complaints in a sample of healthy late-middle aged adults enriched with a family history of Alzheimer disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2017;23(8):617-626. doi:
59.
Pike KE, Cavuoto MG, Li L, Wright BJ, Kinsella GJ. Subjective cognitive decline: Level of risk for future dementia and mild cognitive impairment, a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Neuropsychol Rev. 2022;32(4):703-735. doi:
60.
Jessen F, Wiese B, Bachmann C, et al; German Study on Aging, Cognition and Dementia in Primary Care Patients Study Group. Prediction of dementia by subjective memory impairment: effects of severity and temporal association with cognitive impairment. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(4):414-422. doi:
×