BackgroundÌý
Although substantial evidence suggests that stressful life events predispose to the onset of episodes of depression and anxiety, the essential features of these events that are depressogenic and anxiogenic remain uncertain.
MethodsÌý
High contextual threat stressful life events, assessed in 98Ìý592 person-months from 7322 male and female adult twins ascertained from a population-based registry, were blindly rated on the dimensions of humiliation, entrapment, loss, and danger and their categories. Onsets of pure major depression (MD), pure generalized anxiety syndrome (GAS) (defined as generalized anxiety disorder with a 2-week minimum duration), and mixed MD-GAS episodes were examined using logistic regression.
ResultsÌý
Onsets of pure MD and mixed MD-GAS were predicted by higher ratings of loss and humiliation. Onsets of pure GAS were predicted by higher ratings of loss and danger. High ratings of entrapment predicted only onsets of mixed episodes. The loss categories of death and respondent-initiated separation predicted pure MD but not pure GAS episodes. Events with a combination of humiliation (especially other-initiated separation) and loss were more depressogenic than pure loss events, including death. No sex differences were seen in the prediction of episodes of illness by event categories.
ConclusionsÌý
In addition to loss, humiliating events that directly devalue an individual in a core role were strongly linked to risk for depressive episodes. Event dimensions and categories that predispose to pure MD vs pure GAS episodes can be distinguished with moderate specificity. The event dimensions that preceded mixed MD-GAS episodes were largely the sum of those that preceded pure MD and pure GAS episodes.
ALTHOUGH THE correlation between stressful life events (SLEs)1-3 and the onset of major depression (MD) has been replicated frequently4-8 and is probably causal,9 the attributes that render the events depressogenic are still uncertain.
The concept of loss as the central depressogenic experience can be traced to Freud10 and was first operationalized in SLE research as "exit events."11 An alternative view, influenced by evolutionary theory and animal behavior research, is that reduction of status—forcing the individual into a subordinate position—is the essential depressogenic attribute of events.12 Another perspective, influenced by animal studies,12-14 is that helpless entrapment is the key feature of depressogenic experiences.
Another central question in life event research is the diagnostic specificity of events, as SLEs also precede episodes of anxiety disorders,15-17 alcoholism,18 bulimia,19 and schizophrenia.20 Most studies,15,21,22 however, have examined whether SLEs are divisible into categories of depressogenic vs anxiogenic.
Building on previous methodological developments, Brown and colleagues13 (G. W. Brown, PhD, unpublished manual, Guidelines, Examples and LEDS-2 Notes on Rating for a New Classification Scheme for Humiliation, Loss, and Danger, 1996) proposed a more refined system for rating 4 dimensions of SLEs: loss, humiliation, entrapment, and danger(Table 1). To date, only 2 studies13,23—both with small samples of women—have used this system.
Several previous studies have examined sex differences in sensitivity to the depressogenic effects of standard SLEs.24 Most studies25-28 have reported an increase in sensitivity among women that is either global or restricted to certain events usually involving either interpersonal relations or family. However, analysis of the Epidemiologic Catchment Area study data found greater male sensitivity to the depressogenic effects of marital disruption,29 and, examining standard event categories, our group30 found a stronger depressogenic effect of divorce or separation in men.
In this study, we examine, in 7322 male and female twins from a population-based register, SLEs blindly rated using the 4-dimension system and the onsets in the past year of (1) pure MD episodes, (2) pure episodes of a brief generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) syndrome we call generalized anxiety syndrome (GAS), and (3) mixed MD-GAS episodes. We examine these 2 disorders in part because results of twin studies31-34 suggest that the genetic factors that influence liability to MD and GAD are so closely related that it is environmental risk factors that determine whether a vulnerable individual develops one or the other or both of these syndromes.
Using these more refined event ratings, we seek to (1) define the features of SLEs that predispose to pure MD, pure GAS, and mixed MD-GAS episodes and(2) determine whether men and women differ in the depressogenic and anxiogenic effects of these event dimensions and categories.
Our twin sample was derived from 2 projects with the population-based Virginia Twin Registry35 formed from a systematic review of all birth certificates in the Commonwealth of Virginia. The female-female twin pairs come from birth years 1934 to 1974 and were eligible if both members had previously responded to a mailed questionnaire, which had a response rate of approximately 64%. Eighty-eight percent of our sample was first interviewed face to face between 1987 and 1989 and has subsequently been the subject of 3 additional telephone interview waves. We examine herein results from the second wave of telephone interviews, completed between 1992 and 1994.
The male-male and male-female twin pairs, from birth years 1940 to 1974, were ascertained in 1993.35 We succeeded in interviewing by telephone 72% of those eligible. This sample was followed up in a second wave of interviews (79% of which were completed face to face), the results of which we examine herein. Interviewers were blind to information about the co-twin and previous interviews with the twin. The research protocols were approved by the institutional review board of Virginia Commonwealth University. Signed informed consent was obtained before face-to-face interviews, and verbal consent was obtained before telephone interviews.
We assessed the occurrence during the year before interview of 14 individual symptoms disaggregating the 9 A criteria for DSM-III-R MD.36 For each reported symptom, interviewers probed to ensure that it was due to neither medical illness nor medication use. The interviewer and respondent then aggregated these into syndromes of co-occurring symptoms about which they then asked the months of onset and offset. The diagnosis of MD was made using DSM-III-R criteria excluding criterion B2.
In addition, we asked whether there were times in the past year when participants felt "anxious, nervous, or worried," their "muscles felt tense," or they "felt jumpy or shaky inside." Positive responses to these probes were followed by an assessment of all the individual symptoms of DSM-III-R GAD. For this study, we defined GAS as a syndrome lasting 2 weeks or longer and meeting 6 or more DSM-III-R D criteria for GAD. We used this definition so that our analyses examine symptomatic differences and not duration differences between GAD-like and MD syndromes.
We examined 3 episode types: pure MD, during the MD episode, the participant never met the criteria for GAS; pure GAS, during the GAS episode, the participant never met the criteria for MD; and mixed MD-GAS, during one episode of illness without any 2-week remission, the participant met the criteria for both MD and GAS.
As outlined previously,22 in a separate portion of the interview, we assessed the occurrence by month of 11 "personal" events (assault, divorce or separation, major financial problems, serious housing problems, serious illness or injury, job loss, legal problems, loss of confidant, serious marital problems, robbery, and serious difficulties at work) and 4 classes of "network" events (serious trouble getting along with an individual in the network, a serious personal crisis of someone in the network, and death or serious illness of an individual in the network). All respondents were asked about the occurrence of each of these individual events, which had to be temporally discrete. In an earlier wave of this study, Kendler et al37 assessed the interrater reliability for the occurrence and dating of our SLE categories and found them to be good to excellent, with κ values equaling 0.93 and 0.82, respectively. Each SLE was interviewer rated on the level of long-term contextual threat,38 previously shown to have an interrater and test-retest reliability (by weighted κ39) of 0.41 and 0.69, respectively.22 We define high-threat events as those with a long-term contextual threat score of 3 (high moderate) or 4 (severe).
Coding of Event Dimensions and Categories
All high-threat SLEs were selected for additional nonmutually exclusive ratings of loss, humiliation, entrapment, and danger by using an adaptation of the measures developed by Brown et al.13 Interview audiotapes were reviewed by a team of 1 senior and 2 experienced editors trained according to rating guidelines (G. W. Brown, PhD, unpublished manual, Guidelines, Examples and LEDS-2 Notes on Rating for a New Classification Scheme for Humiliation, Loss, and Danger, 1996). No team member was assigned to rate items in interviews from both individuals in a twin pair. Weekly review meetings were held to maintain rating proficiency, and blind cross-ratings of event items by different interviewers were conducted.
Levels of loss, humiliation, entrapment, and danger were rated contextually using a 5-point scale (0 indicates none present; 1, minor; 2, low moderate; 3, high moderate; and 4, severe) taking into account descriptive information provided in the interview itself, the narrative summary, and the tape-recorded interview. Reports of emotional reactions were ignored. Raters remained blind to the association of rated events with onsets of MD or GAS. Brief definitions of the event dimensions are as follows13:
Loss. Diminution of a sense of connectedness or well-being potentially covering every aspect of life, including a real or realistically imagined loss of a person, material possessions, health, respect in the community, employment, or a cherished idea about self or a close tie.
Humiliation. Feeling devalued in relation to others or to a core sense of self, usually with an element of rejection or a sense of role failure.
Entrapment. Ongoing circumstances of marked difficulty of at least 6 months' duration that the individual can reasonably expect to persist or get worse, with little or no possibility that a resolution can be achieved as a result of anything that might reasonably be done.
Danger. The level of potential future loss, including the chance that a given traumatic event will recur or reflecting a possible sequence of circumstances in which the full threat or dire outcome has yet to be realized.
We rated all 4 dimensions on each high-threat event (differing thereby from Brown et al,13 who rated humiliation and entrapment conditional on high ratings of loss and danger). In addition to scoring these dimensions, we scored 1 of 3 hierarchically specific categories for humiliation and entrapment and 1 of 4 categories for loss (Table 1).
Interrater reliability of the dimensions was assessed blindly by pairs of raters examining 348 individual events with the following weighted κ estimates (and 95% confidence intervals [CIs]): loss, 0.77 (0.72-0.83); humiliation, 0.87 (0.84-0.91); entrapment, 0.92 (0.87-0.96); and danger, 0.79 (0.75-0.83). When both raters agreed on a nonzero dimension score, the weighted κ estimates (and 95% CIs) for the categories were as follows: loss, 0.91 (0.87-0.95)(n = 348); humiliation, 0.99 (0.97-1.00) (n = 107); and entrapment, 0.92 (0.81-1.00)(n = 47).
To model an underlying continuous-time process with data grouped into person-months, discrete-time survival analysis was carried out in SAS PROC GENMOD40 using a complementary log-log link function.41 Advantages of this approach compared with the logit link are that the model is invariant to intervals of different lengths and that the results can be interpreted similarly to a Cox proportional hazards model. Each person-month of observation contained information as to which, if any, SLEs occurred or when an episode of MD or GAS began. Each observation record also included 3 covariates: sex, previous history of MD or GAS within the past year, and the hazard rate (HR) for MD or GAS for that month. When a twin experienced an episode of MD or GAS, the data were censored until the twin was again at risk, having recovered from that episode.
Our person-month files with high-threat SLEs present 2 sources of clustering: multiple observations per person and per twin pair. To handle this complex covariance structure, we used the method of independence estimating equations,42 which are generalized estimating equations with a diagonal working correlation matrix. This method gives consistent estimators of standard errors even when the working correlation structure is incorrect. Although our hypotheses were directional, 2-tailed P values are reported.
For all of the analyses, the sample contained 7322 twins with 98Ìý592 person-months. Of these person-months, 4251 (4.3%) contained a high-threat event. The polycoric correlations of ratings of the 4 SLE dimensions on these 4251 events were low between humiliation and entrapment, humiliation and danger, and loss and danger (≤0.10 for all) but higher between entrapment and danger(0.48), humiliation and loss (0.39), and entrapment and loss (0.32). The mean (SD) ratings for these 4 dimensions were as follows: loss, 2.74 (0.67); humiliation, 0.61 (1.19); entrapment, 0.25 (0.81); and danger, 1.55 (1.38).
The baseline risk per month for a pure MD episode was 0.6%. In the month of occurrence, high-threat events were strongly associated with pure depressive onsets (HR, 10.1; 95% CI, 7.2-14.0; P<.001). Table 2 depicts the impact of event dimensions, in the month of occurrence, on the HRs for pure MD. In months containing high-threat events, the risk for a depressive onset was significantly increased by high ratings for loss (HR, 1.70) and humiliation (HR, 1.44) but not for entrapment (HR, 1.02) or danger (HR, 0.95). We then ran a model containing the 2 strong predictors, and both remained significant (loss: HR, 1.56; 95% CI, 1.34-1.81; and humiliation: HR, 1.29; 95% CI, 1.14-1.46; P<.001 for both), indicating that each dimension independently contributed to predicting depressive onsets.
Next, we examined the impact of event dimensions on risk of a pure depressive onset 1, 2, and 3 months after the event occurrence. None were significant(Table 2).
Mixed depression/anxiety episodes
The baseline risk per month for a mixed depression/anxiety episode was 0.5%. In the month of occurrence, high-threat events were strongly associated with the onset of a mixed depression/anxiety episode (HR, 6.6; 95% CI, 5.3-8.3; P<.001). In months containing high-threat events, the risk for an onset of mixed episodes was significantly increased by high ratings for loss (HR, 1.51) and humiliation (HR, 1.43) but not for entrapment (HR, 1.12) or danger (HR, 1.04) (Table 2). We then ran a model containing the 2 strong predictors, and both remained significant (loss: HR, 1.37; 95% CI, 1.16-1.63; and humiliation: HR, 1.31; 95% CI, 1.13-1.52; P<.001 for both), indicating that each dimension made an independent contribution to the prediction of mixed depression/anxiety onsets. Next, we examined the impact of event dimensions on risk of a mixed episode onset 1, 2, and 3 months after the event occurrence(Table 2). One month after event occurrence, ratings of entrapment (HR, 1.33) and danger (HR, 1.48) significantly predicted onset.
The baseline risk per month for a pure GAS episode was 0.7%. In the month of occurrence, high-threat events were significantly associated with the onset of a pure anxiety episode (HR, 4.5; 95% CI, 3.6-5.6; P<.001). In months containing high-threat events, the risk for an onset of a pure GAS episode was significantly increased by high ratings for loss (HR, 1.35) and danger (HR, 1.18) but not for humiliation (HR, 1.01) or entrapment (HR, 0.98). We then ran a model containing the 2 significant predictors. Loss remained significant (HR, 1.34; 95% CI, 1.12-1.61; P = .001), and danger fell below traditional levels of statistical significance (HR, 1.16; 95% CI, 0.99-1.36; P = .07). Next, we examined the impact of event dimensions on risk of a pure GAS episode onset 1, 2, and 3 months after the event occurrence (Table 2). The only significant finding was an impact of danger ratings 3 months after event occurrence (HR, 1.50).
Loss, humiliation, and entrapment categories
We examined the categories of loss, humiliation, and entrapment for all analyses in which that SLE dimension significantly predicted onsets (Table 3). Compared with SLEs with the category of lesser loss, the specific categories of loss, in the month of event occurrence, carried substantially increased risk for onset of pure MD: death (HR, 2.99), respondent-initiated separation (HR, 3.18), and other key loss (HR, 2.58). Compared with high-threat events with no evidence of humiliation, the risk of a pure depressive onset in the month of event occurrence was substantially increased for those with other-initiated separation (HR, 3.09), with a weaker but still significant effect seen for put down (HR, 1.73).
For mixed depression/anxiety episodes, the pattern of results was similar to that seen for pure MD for the categories of loss and humiliation in the month of event occurrence (Table 3). The only substantial difference was that with mixed episodes, respondent-initiated separation no longer significantly predicted onset. Compared with SLEs with no entrapment rating, mixed depression/anxiety episodes were significantly predicted by long-term sustained entrapment 1 month after event occurrence.
Pure GAS episodes were significantly predicted, in the month of event occurrence, only by other key loss and not by the 2 other categories of loss (death and respondent-initiated separation).
Given the strong and similar predictive roles for loss and humiliation in the month of event occurrence for pure MD and mixed depression/anxiety episodes, we explored the risk for both syndromes collapsed together as a function of the categorizations of both loss and humiliation (Table 4). This examination is limited by the strong interdependencies of these 2 ratings. As expected, all death events and nearly all respondent-initiated separations were rated as having no humiliation. Furthermore, other's delinquency and other-initiated separation were rarely seen with lesser loss. The highest risk of onset (21.6%) occurred when an event was rated as other-initiated separation and other key loss. By contrast, if other-initiated separation was linked with lesser loss, the event carried a much lower risk for onset(2.6%).
Four other combinations carried risks for depressive onsets of approximately 10%. Two of these were pure loss events with zero ratings for humiliation: death and respondent-initiated separation. The other 2 each had other key loss ratings accompanied by humiliation categories of either other's delinquency or put down.
Impact of event dimensions and categories in men vs women
In the month of event occurrence, we examined the interaction between sex and event ratings in the prediction of episode onsets. None of these 12 tests (4 event dimensions times 3 syndromes) were significant.
SLEs AND RISK FOR PURE MD, MIXED MD-GAS, AND PURE GAS
Among high-threat events that were strongly associated with pure depressive onsets (HR, 10.1), we determined whether dimensions of these SLEs particularly reflected risk for MD. We found that both loss and humiliation assess core aspects of depressogenic SLEs. By contrast, levels of entrapment and danger were unassociated with risk of pure MD. When examined together, levels of both loss and humiliation substantially predicted onset of MD, with the impact of loss being more potent. In accord with several previous studies5,7,43 of the SLE-MD relationship, the impact of these events on risk of depression was short-lived, as no significant associations were seen outside the month of event occurrence.
High-threat events, as a class, were also strongly associated with the onset of mixed depression/anxiety syndromes (HR, 6.6). In the month of event occurrence, the risk for such mixed syndromes was also further predicted by high levels of loss and humiliation but not entrapment or danger. However, 1 month after a high-threat event, mixed depression/anxiety episodes were significantly predicted by high levels of entrapment and danger.
The onset of pure GAS episodes was also significantly predicted by high-threat events, although the magnitude of the association (HR, 4.5) was less than that seen with the depressive syndromes. In the month of occurrence of high-threat events, pure GAS episodes were modestly and significantly predicted by loss. Of note, the strength of the association with loss was strongest for pure MD episodes (HR, 1.70), weakest for pure GAS episodes (HR, 1.35), and intermediate for mixed depression/anxiety episodes (HR, 1.51). High ratings of danger also significantly increased risk for a pure anxiety episode in the month of event occurrence and, more strongly, 3 months later.
In addition to rating dimensions of high-threat events, event categories were available for loss, humiliation, and entrapment. A unique pattern of associations was seen among the 3 categories of loss and the 3 types of episodes. Pure MD was significantly associated with death, respondent-initiated separation, and other key loss. Mixed depression/anxiety episodes were significantly associated with death and other key loss. Pure GAS was only significantly predicted by other key loss.
In contrast, the patterns of findings for the event categories of humiliation were identical for the pure depressive and mixed depression/anxiety episodes: significant associations with other-initiated separation and put down. The entrapment categories were examined only for the mixed episodes, in which a significant association was seen only with long-term sustained entrapment.
The most refined analysis we performed was an examination of the risk for depressive onsets (pure and mixed) as a function of ratings of high-threat events on categories of loss and humiliation. By far, the highest risk was seen for events scored as the loss category of other key loss and the humiliation category of other-initiated separation. The probability of a depressive onset associated with this kind of event was more than twice as great as that seen with pure loss associated with death or respondent-initiated separation.
Both previous studies13,23 that used these event ratings examined only depressive onsets and only in women. Like us, Brown et al13 found that the highest risk of depression was associated with combined humiliation and loss events, whereas danger ratings were at most weakly associated with onsets. Unlike us, however, they found that entrapment events were strongly predictive of risk for MD and that death was more strongly associated with depressive onsets than was respondent-initiated separation. Broadhead and Abas23 found that with high-threat events, the rates of depressive onset were highest after the combined categories of humiliation or entrapment, next highest with loss due to death, next with other forms of loss, and lowest after danger events. Given the differences between these studies and ours in sample composition, assessment methods, and diagnostic outcomes, the similarity of results is reassuring.
Previous studies, using a variety of samples and methods, have attempted to distinguish anxiogenic and depressogenic features of SLEs. Results have ranged from no evidence of specificity44,45 to moderate distinctions16,22,46 to high levels of event specificity.15 One study15 examined SLEs before mixed depression/anxiety episodes and found that they were significantly more frequently preceded by severe loss and danger events than were pure depressive or pure anxiety syndromes.
Sex differences in the response to event dimensions
Using relatively refined SLE dimensions, we did not uncover any significant differences in sensitivity to the depressogenic or anxiogenic effects of SLEs in men and women. We were, therefore, unable to contribute to an understanding of the well-documented sex differences in the prevalence of depressive and anxiety disorders.47
We highlight 5 results. First, beginning with high contextual threat events, themselves strongly related to risk of MD and/or GAS, we detected event characteristics that further predicted onsets. The pathogenic effects of SLEs on depressive and anxiety conditions are not fully captured by a single severity dimension.
Second, in addition to loss, humiliation events were strongly linked to risk of depressive episodes. Indeed, the most potent depressogenic events combined elements of both dimensions. Environmental experiences that involve loss of status and elicit "psychobiological programmes of defeat and submission"12(p207) are more depressogenic than those involving solely loss.
Third, we found moderate specificity for the prediction of pure MD vs pure GAS episodes. Using event dimensions, humiliation predicted onsets of pure MD but not pure GAS episodes, and danger predicted pure GAS but not pure MD episodes. Loss was not specific. However, using event categories, death and respondent-initiated separation were specific for pure depressive episodes. These results are consistent with findings31-33 from twin modeling that suggest that the correlation in environmental risk factors for MD and GAD is positive but only moderate in magnitude.
Fourth, the event dimensions that preceded mixed depressive/anxiety episodes were approximately the sum of those that preceded pure MD (loss and humiliation) and pure GAS (danger). The exception to this pattern was the significant association of high levels of entrapment to onsets of mixed episodes.
Fifth, although the effects of loss and humiliation on risk of episode onset were restricted to the month of event occurrence, the impact of danger (and perhaps entrapment) on risk of illness was more prolonged.
Five limitations are noteworthy. First, because our sample consisted entirely of white twins from Virginia, these findings may not extrapolate to other ethnic groups or geographic regions. Second, our analyses assumed that when SLEs occurred in the same month as depressive onsets, the relationship between these 2 was causal. In another section of our interview, we inquired of twins with depressive onsets in the past year whether anything happened to precipitate their episodes. Using data from the first 2 waves of our study,37 we examined interviews from 96 twins who reported a severe SLE and a depressive onset in the same month. In 84% of them, the twin responded with the same previously reported SLE. In another 11%, they reported a different SLE that had also co-occurred in the same month in an understandable sequence of events including the severe SLE. We replicated these results for a later wave,22 in which a review of 102 similar cases revealed none in which the depressive onset plausibly caused the SLE. Although our data are retrospective (over the recall interval of up to 1 year), our analyses support the assumption of a largely causal relationship between the SLE and the onset of MD when they occur in the same month. Furthermore, analyses based on a co-twin control design suggest that most of the SLE-MD relationship is a causal one.9 Third, unlike the methods of Brown et al13 for rating event dimensions and categories by committee, they were, in our study, performed by a small team of editors with high interrater reliability. Given our sample size, rating each SLE by committee would have been unfeasible. Fourth, our diagnostic category of GAS was unconventional—although the minimum duration was that required for GAD when it was first proposed.48 We kept differences between GAS and MD to the level of symptoms, thereby not confounding our analyses with differences in duration. We repeated our analysis using a 1-month minimum duration for GAS—as required in DSM-III49—and found only modest changes.
Corresponding author and reprints: Kenneth S. Kendler, MD, Department of Psychiatry, PO Box 980126, Richmond, VA 23298-0126.
Submitted for publication August 5, 2002; final revision received December 20, 2002; accepted January 9, 2003.
This work was supported by grants MH-40828, AA-09095, AA-00236, and MH/AA/DA-49492 from the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md, and a Young Investigator Award from the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression, Great Neck, NY (Dr Hettema).
We acknowledge the contribution of the Virginia Twin Registry, now part of the Mid-Atlantic Twin Registry (MATR), to the ascertainment of individuals for this study. The MATR has received support from the National Institutes of Health, the Carman Trust, and the W. M. Keck Foundation (Los Angeles, Calif), John Templeton Foundation (Radnor, Pa), and The Robert Wood Johnson Foundation (Princeton, NJ).
1.Dohrenwend
ÌýBSedDohrenwend
ÌýBPedÌýStressful Life Events and Their Context.Ìý New Brunswick, NJ Rutgers University Press1984;
2.Thoits
ÌýPAÌýDimensions of life events that influence psychological distress: an evaluation and synthesis of the literature.ÌýKaplan
ÌýHBed.ÌýPsychosocial Stress: Trends in Theory and Research New York, NY Academic Press1983;33-Ìý102
3.Kessler
ÌýRCÌýThe effects of stressful life events on depression.ÌýÌýAnnu Rev Psychol. 1997;48191-Ìý214
4.Paykel
ÌýESÌýContribution of life events to causation of psychatric illness.ÌýÌýPsychol Med. 1978;8245-Ìý253
5.Brown
ÌýGWHarris
ÌýTOÌýSocial Origins of Depression: A Study of Psychiatric Disorder in Women.Ìý London, England Tavistock1978;
6.Costello
ÌýCGÌýSocial factors associated with depression: a retrospective community study.ÌýÌýPsychol Med. 1982;12329-Ìý339
7.Surtees
ÌýPGMiller
ÌýPMIngham
ÌýJGKreitman
ÌýNBRennie
ÌýDSashidharan
ÌýSPÌýLife events and the onset of affective disorder: a longitudinal general population study.ÌýÌýJ Affect Disord. 1986;1037-Ìý50
8.Bebbington
ÌýPESturt
ÌýETennant
ÌýCHurry
ÌýJÌýMisfortune and resilience: a community study of women.ÌýÌýPsychol Med. 1984;14347-Ìý363
9.Kendler
ÌýKSKarkowski
ÌýLMPrescott
ÌýCAÌýCausal relationship between stressful life events and the onset of major depression.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1999;156837-Ìý841
10.Freud
ÌýSÌýMourning and Melancholia.ÌýStrachey
ÌýJed.ÌýComplete Psychological Works. 14 London, England Hogarth Press1957;
11.Paykel
ÌýESMyers
ÌýJKDienelt
ÌýMNKlerman
ÌýGLLindenthal
ÌýJJPepper
ÌýMPÌýLife events and depression: a controlled study.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1969;21753-Ìý760
12.Gilbert
ÌýPÌýDepression: The Evolution of Powerlessness.Ìý New York, NY Guilford Publications1992;
13.Brown
ÌýGWHarris
ÌýTOHepworth
ÌýCÌýLoss, humiliation and entrapment among women developing depression: a patient and non-patient comparison.ÌýÌýPsychol Med. 1995;257-Ìý21
14.Abramson
ÌýLSeligman
ÌýMTeasdale
ÌýJÌýLearned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation.ÌýÌýJ Abnorm Psychol. 1978;8749-Ìý74
15.Finlay-Jones
ÌýRBrown
ÌýGWÌýTypes of stressful life events and the onset of anxiety and depressive disorders.ÌýÌýPsychol Med. 1981;11803-Ìý815
16.Barrett
ÌýJEÌýThe relationship of life events to the onset of neurotic disorders.ÌýBarrett
ÌýJERose
ÌýRMKlerman
ÌýGLeds.ÌýStress and Mental Disorder New York, NY Raven Press1979;87-Ìý109
17.Faravelli
ÌýCPallanti
ÌýSÌýRecent life events and panic disorder.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1989;146622-Ìý626
18.Gorman
ÌýDMPeters
ÌýTJÌýTypes of life events and onset of alcohol dependence.ÌýÌýBr J Addict. 1990;8571-Ìý79
19.Welch
ÌýSLDoll
ÌýHAFairburn
ÌýCGÌýLife events and the onset of bulimia nervosa: a controlled study.ÌýÌýPsychol Med. 1997;27515-Ìý522
20.Day
ÌýRÌýSchizophrenia.ÌýBrown
ÌýGWHarris
ÌýTOeds.ÌýLife Events and Illness New York, NY Guilford Publications1989;113-Ìý137
21.Uhlenhuth
ÌýEHPaykel
ÌýESÌýSymptom configuration and life events.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1973;28744-Ìý748
22.Kendler
ÌýKSKarkowski
ÌýLPrescott
ÌýCAÌýStressful life events and major depression: risk period, long-term contextual threat and diagnostic specificity.ÌýÌýJ Nerv Ment Dis. 1998;186661-Ìý669
23.Broadhead
ÌýJCAbas
ÌýMEÌýLife events, difficulties and depression among women in an uban setting in Zimbabwe.ÌýÌýPsychol Med. 1998;2829-Ìý38
24.Nolen-Hoeksema
ÌýSÌýSex Differences in Depression.Ìý Palo Alto, Calif Stanford University Press1990;
25.Van Os
ÌýJJones
ÌýPBÌýEarly risk factors and adult person–environment relationships in affective disorder.ÌýÌýPsychol Med. 1999;291055-Ìý1067
26.Wagner
ÌýBMCompas
ÌýBEÌýGender, instrumentality, and expressivity: moderators of the relation between stress and psychological symptoms during adolescence.ÌýÌýAm J Community Psychol. 1990;18383-Ìý406
27.Nazroo
ÌýJYEdwards
ÌýACBrown
ÌýGWÌýGender differences in the onset of depression following a shared life event: a study of couples.ÌýÌýPsychol Med. 1997;279-Ìý19
28.Kessler
ÌýRCMcLeod
ÌýJDÌýSex differences in vulnerability to undesirable life events.ÌýÌýAm Sociol Rev. 1984;49620-Ìý631
29.Bruce
ÌýMLKim
ÌýKMÌýDifferences in the effects of divorce on major depresison in men and women.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1992;149914-Ìý917
30.Kendler
ÌýKSThornton
ÌýLMPrescott
ÌýCAÌýGender differences in the rates of exposure to stressful life events and sensitivity to their depressogenic effects.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 2001;158587-Ìý593
31.Kendler
ÌýKSNeale
ÌýMCKessler
ÌýRCHeath
ÌýACEaves
ÌýLJÌýGeneralized anxiety disorder in women: a population-based twin study.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49267-Ìý272
32.Kendler
ÌýKSNeale
ÌýMCKessler
ÌýRCHeath
ÌýACEaves
ÌýLJÌýMajor depression and generalized anxiety disorder: same genes, (partly) different environments?ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49716-Ìý722
33.Roy
ÌýM-ANeale
ÌýMCPedersen
ÌýNLMathe
ÌýAAKendler
ÌýKSÌýA twin study of generalized anxiety disorder and major depression.ÌýÌýPsychol Med. 1995;251037-Ìý1049
34.Kendler
ÌýKSÌýMajor depression and generalised anxiety disorder: same genes (partly) different environments—revisited.ÌýÌýBr J Psychiatry. 1996;16868-Ìý75
35.Kendler
ÌýKSPrescott
ÌýCAÌýA population-based twin study of lifetime major depression in men and women.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;5639-Ìý44
36.American Psychiatric Association,ÌýDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition.Ìý Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1987;
37.Kendler
ÌýKSKessler
ÌýRCWalters
ÌýEEMacLean
ÌýCJSham
ÌýPCNeale
ÌýMCHeath
ÌýACEaves
ÌýLJÌýStressful life events, genetic liability and onset of an episode of major depression in women.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1995;152833-Ìý842
38.Brown
ÌýGWÌýLife events and measurement.ÌýBrown
ÌýGWHarris
ÌýTOeds.ÌýLife Events and Illness New York, NY Guilford Publications1989;3-Ìý45
39.Fleiss
ÌýJSCohen
ÌýJEverett
ÌýBSÌýLarge sample standard errors of kappa and weighted kappa.ÌýÌýPsychol Bull. 1969;72323-Ìý327
40.SAS Institute Inc,ÌýSAS/STAT® User's Guide, Version 8.Ìý Cary, NC SAS Institute Inc2000;
41.ÌýSurvival Analysis Using the SAS System: A Practical Guide.Ìý2nd Cary, NC SAS Institute Inc1995;292
42.Zeger
ÌýSLLiang
ÌýKYÌýLongitudinal data analysis for discrete and continuous outcomes.ÌýÌýBiometrics. 1986;42121-Ìý130
43.Bebbington
ÌýPETennant
ÌýCHurry
ÌýJÌýAdversity and the nature of psychiatric disorder in the community.ÌýÌýJ Affect Disord. 1981;3345-Ìý366
44.Newman
ÌýSCBland
ÌýRCÌýLife events and the 1-year prevalence of major depressive episode, generalized anxiety disorder, and panic disorder in a community sample.ÌýÌýCompr Psychiatry. 1994;3576-Ìý82
45.Murray
ÌýLGBlackburn
ÌýIMÌýPersonality differences in patients with depressive illness and anxiety neurosis.ÌýÌýActa Psychiatr Scand. 1974;50183-Ìý191
46.Miller
ÌýPMIngham
ÌýJGÌýDimensions of experience and symptomatology.ÌýÌýJ Psychosom Res. 1985;29475-Ìý488
47.Kessler
ÌýRCMcGonagle
ÌýKAZhao
ÌýSNelson
ÌýCBHughes
ÌýMEshleman
ÌýSWittchen
ÌýH-UKendler
ÌýKSÌýLifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;518-Ìý19
48.Spitzer
ÌýRLEndicott
ÌýJRobins
ÌýEÌýResearch Diagnostic Criteria for a Selected Group of Functional Disorders.Ìý2nd New York New York Psychiatric Institute1975;
49.American Psychiatric Association,ÌýDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition.Ìý Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1980;