ÌÇÐÄvlog

[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.Ìý

Alcoholic beverage pictures task design and stimuli samples.

Figure 2.Ìý

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) results during alcoholic beverage picture trials relative to nonalcoholic beverage picture trials. Orange indicates where teens with alcohol use disorder (n = 15) had more response than control subjects (n = 15) to alcoholic beverage pictures. Blue shows where controls had more response to alcoholic beverage pictures (group P<.05; clusters, >515 µL). Numbers refer to axial slice positions. The fMRI results are displayed on averaged anatomic brain maps.

Figure 3.Ìý

Blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) response signal contrast in the right precuneus/posterior cingulate region during exposure to alcoholic beverage pictures relative to nonalcoholic beverage pictures plotted as a function of drinks consumed per month for adolescents with alcohol use disorder (n = 15; r = 0.76 [P<.001]).

Table 1.Ìý
Characteristics of Adolescent Participants*
Table 2.Ìý
Regions Where Controls and Teens With AUD Showed Significant Differences in BOLD Response While Viewing Alcoholic Beverage Pictures Relative to Neutral Beverage Pictures*
Table 3.Ìý
Relationship Between Mild Desires to Drink and BOLD Response to Alcoholic Beverage Pictures in Adolescents With AUD*
1.
Tiffany ÌýSTÌýA cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes.ÌýÌýPsychol Rev. 1990;97147-Ìý168
2.
Tiffany ÌýSTConklin ÌýCAÌýA cognitive processing model of alcohol craving and compulsive alcohol use.ÌýÌý´¡»å»å¾±³¦³Ù¾±´Ç²Ô. 2000;95suppl 2S145-ÌýS153
3.
Anton ÌýRFDrobes ÌýDJÌýClinical measurement of craving in addiction.ÌýÌýPsychiatr Ann. 1998;28553-Ìý560
4.
McDonough ÌýBEWarren ÌýCAÌýEffects of 12-h tobacco deprivation on event-related potentials elicited by visual smoking cues.ÌýÌý±Ê²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²¹°ù³¾²¹³¦´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 2001;154282-Ìý291
5.
Monti ÌýPMRohsenow ÌýDJHutchison ÌýKEÌýToward bridging the gap between biological, psychobiological and psychosocial models of alcohol craving.ÌýÌý´¡»å»å¾±³¦³Ù¾±´Ç²Ô. 2000;95suppl 2S229-ÌýS236
6.
Monti ÌýPMBinkoff ÌýJAAbrams ÌýDBZwick ÌýWRNirenberg ÌýTDLiepman ÌýMRÌýReactivity of alcoholics and nonalcoholics to drinking cues.ÌýÌýJ Abnorm Psychol. 1987;96122-Ìý126
7.
Rohsenow ÌýDJMonti ÌýPMAbrams ÌýDBRubonis ÌýAVNiaura ÌýRSSirota ÌýADColby ÌýSMÌýCue elicited urge to drink and salivation in alcoholics: relationship to individual differences and relapse.ÌýÌýAdv Behav Res Ther. 1992;14195-Ìý210
8.
Stormark ÌýKMLaberg ÌýJCNordby ÌýHHugdahl ÌýKÌýAlcoholics' selective attention to alcohol stimuli: automated processing?ÌýÌýJ Stud Alcohol. 2000;6118-Ìý23
9.
McCusker ÌýCGBrown ÌýKÌýCue-exposure to alcohol-associated stimuli reduces autonomic reactivity, but not craving and anxiety, in dependent drinkers.ÌýÌýAlcohol Alcohol. 1995;30319-Ìý327
10.
Stormark ÌýKMField ÌýNPHugdahl ÌýKHorowitz ÌýMÌýSelective processing of visual alcohol cues in abstinent alcoholics: an approach-avoidance conflict?ÌýÌýAddict Behav. 1997;22509-Ìý519
11.
Johnsen ÌýBHLaberg ÌýJCCox ÌýWMVaksdal ÌýAHugdahl ÌýKÌýAlcoholic subjects' attentional bias in the processing of alcohol-related words.ÌýÌýPsychol Addict Behav. 1994;8111-Ìý115
12.
Stetter ÌýFAckermann ÌýKBizer ÌýAStraube ÌýERMann ÌýKÌýEffects of disease-related cues in alcoholic inpatients: results of a controlled "alcohol Stroop" study.ÌýÌýAlcohol Clin Exp Res. 1995;19593-Ìý599
13.
Myers ÌýMGBrown ÌýSAMott ÌýMAÌýCoping as a predictor of adolescent substance abuse treatment outcome.ÌýÌýJ Subst Abuse. 1993;515-Ìý29
14.
Hommer ÌýDWÌýFunctional imaging of craving.ÌýÌýAlcohol Res Health. 1999;23187-Ìý196
15.
Modell ÌýJGMountz ÌýJMÌýFocal cerebral blood flow change during craving for alcohol measured by SPECT.ÌýÌýJ Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1995;715-Ìý22
16.
Breiter ÌýHCGollub ÌýRLWeisskoff ÌýRMKennedy ÌýDNMakris ÌýNBerke ÌýJDGoodman ÌýJMKantor ÌýHLGastfriend ÌýDRRiorden ÌýJPMathew ÌýRTRosen ÌýBRHyman ÌýSEÌýAcute effects of cocaine on human brain activity and emotion.ÌýÌý±·±ð³Ü°ù´Ç²Ô. 1997;19591-Ìý611
17.
Grant ÌýSLondon ÌýEDNewlin ÌýDBVillemagne ÌýVLLiu ÌýXContoreggi ÌýCPhillips ÌýRLKimes ÌýASMargolin ÌýAÌýActivation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving.ÌýÌýProc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;9312040-Ìý12045
18.
Maas ÌýLCLukas ÌýSEKaufman ÌýMJWeiss ÌýRDDaniels ÌýSLRogers ÌýVWKukes ÌýTJRenshaw ÌýPFÌýFunctional magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activation during cue-induced cocaine craving.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1998;155124-Ìý126
19.
George ÌýMSAnton ÌýRFBloomer ÌýCTeneback ÌýCDrobes ÌýDJLorberbaum ÌýJPNahas ÌýZVincent ÌýDJÌýActivation of prefrontal cortex and anterior thalamus in alcoholic subjects on exposure to alcohol-specific cues.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58345-Ìý352
20.
Volkow ÌýNDWang ÌýG-JFowler ÌýJSHitzemann ÌýRAngrist ÌýBGatley ÌýSJLogan ÌýJDing ÌýY-SPappas ÌýNÌýAssociation of methylphenidate-induced craving with changes in right striato-orbitofrontal metabolism in cocaine abusers: implications in addiction.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1999;15619-Ìý26
21.
Childress ÌýARMozley ÌýPDMcElgin ÌýWFitzgerald ÌýJReivich ÌýMO'Brien ÌýCPÌýLimbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1999;15611-Ìý18Google Scholar
22.
Garavan ÌýHPankiewicz ÌýJBloom ÌýACho ÌýJKSperry ÌýLRoss ÌýTJSalmeron ÌýBJRisinger ÌýRKelley ÌýDStein ÌýEAÌýCue-induced cocaine craving: neuroanatomical specificity for drug users and drug stimuli.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 2000;1571789-Ìý1798
23.
Sell ÌýLAMorris ÌýJSBearn ÌýJFrackowiak ÌýRSFriston ÌýKJDolan ÌýRJÌýNeural responses associated with cue evoked emotional states and heroin in opiate addicts.ÌýÌýDrug Alcohol Depend. 2000;60207-Ìý216
24.
Wang ÌýGJVolkow ÌýNDFowler ÌýJSCervany ÌýPHitzemann ÌýRJPappas ÌýNRWong ÌýCTFelder ÌýCÌýRegional brain metabolic activation during craving elicited by recall of previous drug experiences.ÌýÌýLife Sci. 1999;64775-Ìý784
25.
Kilts ÌýCDSchweitzer ÌýJBQuinn ÌýCKGross ÌýREFaber ÌýTLMuhammad ÌýFEly ÌýTDHoffman ÌýJMDrexler ÌýKPÌýNeural activity related to drug craving in cocaine addiction.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58334-Ìý341
26.
Tapert ÌýSFBrown ÌýGGBaratta ÌýMVBrown ÌýSAÌýfMRI BOLD response to alcohol stimuli in alcohol dependent young women.ÌýÌýAddict Behav. In press
27.
Weinstein ÌýAFeldtkeller ÌýBMalizia ÌýAWilson ÌýSBailey ÌýJNutt ÌýDJÌýIntegrating the cognitive and physiological aspects of craving.ÌýÌýJ Psychopharmacol. 1998;1231-Ìý38
28.
Martin ÌýCKaczynski ÌýNMaisto ÌýSBukstein ÌýOMoss ÌýHÌýPatterns of DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence symptoms in adolescent drinkers.ÌýÌýJ Stud Alcohol. 1995;56672-Ìý680
29.
Brown ÌýSATapert ÌýSFTate ÌýSRAbrantes ÌýAMÌýThe role of alcohol in adolescent relapse and outcome.ÌýÌýJ Psychoactive Drugs. 2000;32107-Ìý115
30.
Martin ÌýSESnyder ÌýLBHamilton ÌýMFleming-Milici ÌýFSlater ÌýMDStacy ÌýAChen ÌýMJGrube ÌýJWÌýAlcohol advertising and youth.ÌýÌýAlcohol Clin Exp Res. 2002;26900-Ìý906
31.
Rice ÌýJPReich ÌýTBucholz ÌýKKNeuman ÌýRJFishman ÌýRRochberg ÌýNHesselbrock ÌýVMNurnberger ÌýJI ÌýJrSchuckit ÌýMABegleiter ÌýHÌýComparison of direct interview and family history diagnoses of alcohol dependence.ÌýÌýAlcohol Clin Exp Res. 1995;191018-Ìý1023
32.
Brown ÌýSAMyers ÌýMGLippke ÌýLTapert ÌýSFStewart ÌýDGVik ÌýPWÌýPsychometric evaluation of the Customary Drinking and Drug Use Record (CDDR): a measure of adolescent alcohol and drug involvement.ÌýÌýJ Stud Alcohol. 1998;59427-Ìý438
33.
Shaffer ÌýDFisher ÌýPLucas ÌýCPDulcan ÌýMKSchwab-Stone ÌýMEÌýNIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses.ÌýÌýJ Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;3928-Ìý38
34.
Brown ÌýSAGleghorn ÌýAASchuckit ÌýMAMyers ÌýMGMott ÌýMAÌýConduct disorder among adolescent alcohol and drug abusers.ÌýÌýJ Stud Alcohol. 1996;57314-Ìý324
35.
Myers ÌýMGStewart ÌýDGBrown ÌýSAÌýProgression from conduct disorder to antisocial personality disorder following treatment for adolescent substance abuse.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1998;155479-Ìý485
36.
Brown ÌýSAVik ÌýPWCreamer ÌýVAÌýCharacteristics of relapse following adolescent substance abuse treatment.ÌýÌýAddict Behav. 1989;14291-Ìý300
37.
Hollingshead ÌýABÌýTwo-Factor Index of Social Position.Ìý New Haven, Conn Yale University Press1965;
38.
Achenbach ÌýTMÌýManual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile.Ìý Burlington University of Vermont Dept of Psychiatry1991;
39.
American Psychiatric Association,ÌýDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition.Ìý Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1987;
40.
American Psychiatric Association,ÌýDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition.Ìý Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
41.
Stewart ÌýDGBrown ÌýSAÌýWithdrawal and dependency symptoms among adolescent alcohol and drug abusers.ÌýÌý´¡»å»å¾±³¦³Ù¾±´Ç²Ô. 1995;90627-Ìý635
42.
Sobell ÌýLCSobell ÌýMBÌýTimeline Follow-back: a technique for assessing self-reported alcohol consumption.ÌýRaye ÌýZLitten ÌýJPAedÌýMeasuring Alcohol Consumption: Psychosocial and Biochemical Methods. Totowa, NJ Humana Press Inc1992;41-Ìý72
43.
Wechsler ÌýDÌýManual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children III.Ìý San Antonio, Tex Psychological Corp1993;
44.
Sattler ÌýJMÌýAssessment of Children, Third Edition Revised, Updated.Ìý San Diego, Calif JM Sattler1992;
45.
Beck ÌýATÌýBeck Depression Inventory (BDI).Ìý San Antonio, Tex Psychological Corp1978;
46.
Spielberger ÌýCDGorsuch ÌýRLLushene ÌýREÌýManual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.Ìý Palo Alto, Calif Consulting Psychologists Press1970;
47.
Glenville ÌýMBroughton ÌýRÌýReliability of the Stanford Sleepiness Scale compared to short duration performance tests and the Wilkinson Auditory Vigilance Task.ÌýÌýAdv Biosci. 1978;21235-Ìý244
48.
Chick ÌýJHowlett ÌýHMorgan ÌýMYRitson ÌýBÌýUnited Kingdom Multicentre Acamprosate Study (UKMAS): a 6-month prospective study of acamprosate versus placebo in preventing relapse after withdrawal from alcohol.ÌýÌýAlcohol Alcohol. 2000;35176-Ìý187
49.
Love ÌýAJames ÌýDWillner ÌýPÌýA comparison of two alcohol craving questionnaires.ÌýÌý´¡»å»å¾±³¦³Ù¾±´Ç²Ô. 1998;931091-Ìý1102
50.
Wong ÌýECLuh ÌýWMBuxton ÌýRBFrank ÌýLRÌýSingle slab high resolution 3D whole brain imaging using spiral FSE.ÌýÌýProc Intl Soc Magn Reson Med. 2000;8683
51.
Meyer ÌýCHHu ÌýBSNishimura ÌýDGMacovski ÌýAÌýFast spiral coronary artery imaging.ÌýÌýMagn Reson Med. 1992;28202-Ìý213
52.
Noll ÌýDCPauly ÌýJMMeyer ÌýCHNishimura ÌýDGMacovski ÌýAÌýDeblurring for non-2D Fourier transform magnetic resonance imaging.ÌýÌýMagn Reson Med. 1992;25319-Ìý333
53.
Cox ÌýRWÌýAFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages.ÌýÌýComput Biomed Res. 1996;29162-Ìý173
54.
Cox ÌýRWJesmanowicz ÌýAÌýReal-time 3D image registration for functional MRI.ÌýÌýMagn Reson Med. 1999;421014-Ìý1018
55.
Bandettini ÌýPAJesmanowicz ÌýAWong ÌýECHyde ÌýJSÌýProcessing strategies for time-course data sets in functional MRI of the human brain.ÌýÌýMagn Reson Med. 1993;30161-Ìý173
56.
Talairach ÌýJTournoux ÌýPÌýCoplanar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: Three-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging.Ìý New York, NY Thieme-Stratton Inc1988;
57.
Forman ÌýSDCohen ÌýJDFitzgerald ÌýMEddy ÌýWFMintun ÌýMANoll ÌýDCÌýImproved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold.ÌýÌýMagn Reson Med. 1995;33636-Ìý647
58.
Ward ÌýBDÌýSimultaneous Inference for fMRI Data.Ìý Milwaukee, Wis Biophysics Research Institute, Medical College ofWisconsin1997;
59.
Schuckit ÌýMAGold ÌýEOCroot ÌýKFinn ÌýPPolich ÌýJÌýP300 latency after ethanol ingestion in sons of alcoholics and in controls.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1988;24310-Ìý315
60.
Schuckit ÌýMASmith ÌýTLÌýThe relationships of a family history of alcohol dependence, a low level of response to alcohol and six domains of life functioning to the development of alcohol use disorders.ÌýÌýJ Stud Alcohol. 2000;61827-Ìý835
61.
Ehlers ÌýCLSchuckit ÌýMAÌýEvaluation of EEG alpha activity in sons of alcoholics.ÌýÌýNeuropsychopharmacology. 1991;4199-Ìý205
62.
Hada ÌýMPorjesz ÌýBChorlian ÌýDBBegleiter ÌýHPolich ÌýJÌýAuditory P3a deficits in male subjects at high risk for alcoholism.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 2001;49726-Ìý738
63.
Giedd ÌýJNVaituzis ÌýACHamburger ÌýSDLange ÌýNRajapakse ÌýJCKaysen ÌýDVauss ÌýYCRapoport ÌýJLÌýQuantitative MRI of the temporal lobe, amygdala, and hippocampus in normal human development: ages 4-18 years.ÌýÌýJ Comp Neurol. 1996;366223-Ìý230
64.
Schlaepfer ÌýTEHarris ÌýGJTien ÌýAYPeng ÌýLLee ÌýSPearlson ÌýGDÌýStructural differences in the cerebral cortex of healthy female and male subjects: a magnetic resonance imaging study.ÌýÌýPsychiatry Res. 1995;61129-Ìý135
65.
Caviness ÌýVS ÌýJrKennedy ÌýDNRichelme ÌýCRademacher ÌýJFilipek ÌýPAÌýThe human brain age 7-11 years: a volumetric analysis based on magnetic resonance images.ÌýÌýCereb Cortex. 1996;6726-Ìý736
66.
Killgore ÌýWDOki ÌýMYurgelun-Todd ÌýDAÌýSex-specific developmental changes in amygdala responses to affective faces.ÌýÌýNeuroreport. 2001;12427-Ìý433
67.
Wexler ÌýBEGottschalk ÌýCHFulbright ÌýRKProhovnik ÌýILacadie ÌýCMRounsaville ÌýBJGore ÌýJCÌýFunctional magnetic resonance imaging of cocaine craving.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 2001;15886-Ìý95
68.
Daglish ÌýMRCWeinstein ÌýAMalazia ÌýALWilson ÌýSMelichar ÌýJKBritten ÌýSBrewer ÌýCLingford-Hughes ÌýAMyles ÌýJSGrasby ÌýPNutt ÌýDJÌýChanges in regional cerebral blood flow elicited by craving memories in abstinent opiate-dependent subjects.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 2001;1581680-Ìý1686
69.
Schneider ÌýFHabel ÌýUWagner ÌýMFranke ÌýPSalloum ÌýJBShah ÌýNJToni ÌýISulzbach ÌýCHonig ÌýKMaier ÌýWGaebel ÌýWZilles ÌýKÌýSubcortical correlates of craving in recently abstinent alcoholic patients.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 2001;1581075-Ìý1083
70.
Gaillard ÌýWDHertz-Pannier ÌýLMott ÌýSHBarnett ÌýASLeBihan ÌýDTheodore ÌýWHÌýFunctional anatomy of cognitive development: fMRI of verbal fluency in children and adults.ÌýÌýNeurology. 2000;54180-Ìý185
71.
Thomas ÌýKMKing ÌýSWFranzen ÌýPLWelsh ÌýTFBerkowitz ÌýALNoll ÌýDCBirmaher ÌýVCasey ÌýBJÌýA developmental functional MRI study of spatial working memory.ÌýÌýNeuroimage. 1999;10pt 1327-Ìý338
72.
Saenz ÌýMBuracas ÌýGTBoynton ÌýGMÌýGlobal effects of feature-based attention in human visual cortex.ÌýÌýNat Neurosci. 2002;5631-Ìý632
73.
Bechara ÌýADamasio ÌýHÌýDecision-making and addiction, I: impaired activation of somatic states in substance dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences.ÌýÌýNeuropsychologia. 2002;401675-Ìý1689
74.
Elliott ÌýRRees ÌýGDolan ÌýRJÌýVentromedial prefrontal cortex mediates guessing.ÌýÌýNeuropsychologia. 1999;37403-Ìý411
75.
Gawin ÌýFHÌýThe scientific exegesis of desire: neuroimaging crack craving.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58342-Ìý344
76.
Casswell ÌýSZhang ÌýJ-FSilva ÌýPAÌýAlcohol in the mass media and drinking by adolescents: a longitudinal study.ÌýÌý´¡»å»å¾±³¦³Ù¾±´Ç²Ô. 1994;891255-Ìý1263
77.
Wyllie ÌýAZhang ÌýJFCasswell ÌýSÌýResponses to televised alcohol advertisements associated with drinking behaviour of 10-17-year-olds.ÌýÌý´¡»å»å¾±³¦³Ù¾±´Ç²Ô. 1998;93361-Ìý371
78.
Johnson ÌýJGCohen ÌýPSmailes ÌýEMKasen ÌýSBrook ÌýJSÌýTelevision viewing and aggressive behavior during adolescence and adulthood.ÌýÌýScience. 2002;2952468-Ìý2471
79.
Slater ÌýMDRouner ÌýDMurphy ÌýKBeauvais ÌýFVan Leuven ÌýJDomenech-Rodriguez ÌýMMÌýAdolescent counterarguing of TV beer advertisements: evidence for effectiveness of alcohol education and critical viewing discussions.ÌýÌýJ Drug Educ. 1996;26143-Ìý158
80.
Agostinelli ÌýGGrube ÌýJWÌýAlcohol counter-advertising and the media: a review of recent research.ÌýÌýAlcohol Res Health. 2002;2615-Ìý21
Original Article
JulyÌý2003

Neural Response to Alcohol Stimuli in Adolescents With Alcohol Use Disorder

Author Affiliations

From the Psychology Service (Drs Tapert, G. G. Brown, and S. A. Brown), Radiology Service (Dr Frank), and Psychiatry Service (Drs Paulus and Meloy), Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System, and the Departments of Psychiatry (Drs Tapert, G. G. Brown, Paulus, and S. A. Brown and Mr Cheung), Radiology (Dr Frank), and Psychology (Ms Schweinsburg and Dr S. A. Brown), University of California–San Diego.

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(7):727-735. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.7.727
Abstract

BackgroundÌý Cue reactivity studies in alcohol-dependent adults have shown atypical physiological, cognitive, and neural responses to alcohol-related stimuli that differ from the responses of light drinkers. Cue reactivity and its neural substrates are unclear in youth. We hypothesized that teens with alcohol use disorder would show greater brain response than nonabusing teens to alcohol images relative to neutral beverage images in limbic and frontal brain regions.

MethodsÌý We tested the hypotheses in a cross-sectional functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Adolescents aged 14 to 17 were recruited from local high schools. Teens with alcohol use disorders (n = 15) and demographically similar infrequent drinkers (n = 15) met strict exclusion criteria (no left-handedness or neurological, other psychiatric, or other substance use disorders). Diagnoses were determined by means of structured and semistructured clinical interviews. Subjects were shown pictures of alcoholic and nonalcoholic beverage advertisements during blood oxygen level–dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging. Self-reports of craving were obtained before and after cue exposure.

ResultsÌý Teens with alcohol use disorders showed substantially greater brain activation to alcoholic beverage pictures than control youths, predominantly in the left anterior, limbic, and visual system areas (P<.05; cluster threshold, 515 µL). The degree of brain response to the alcohol pictures was highest in youths who consumed more drinks per month and reported greater desires to drink.

ConclusionsÌý These results confirm previous studies by demonstrating an association between the urge to drink alcohol and blood oxygen use in areas of the brain previously linked to reward, desire, positive affect, and episodic recall. This study extends this relationship to adolescents with relatively brief drinking histories using visual alcohol stimuli, and suggests a neural basis for response to alcohol advertisements in youths with drinking problems.

DRUG CRAVING is often described as the subjective experience of an intense desire for an addictive substance.1,2 It may occupy cognitive resources and influence substance use decisions,3 but it is difficult to measure objectively. Cue reactivity is an observable correlate of craving4 that has been demonstrated in alcohol-dependent adults5-7 through physiological changes such as increased heart rate or salivation on exposure to alcohol-related words, pictures, scents, tactile cues, or imaginal stimuli.6,8,9 Alcohol-dependent adults show difficulty shifting attention away from alcohol-related stimuli,10 and, consequently, substance cues can interfere with the deployment of effective coping responses.11,12 Although cue reactivity has not been studied in adolescents, substance-dependent youths show difficulty implementing coping skills when substance cues are salient.13

Knowledge about the neural systems subserving cue reactivity and craving has expanded recently.14 Alcohol-dependent adults given small amounts of alcohol showed increased blood flow in the right caudate nucleus that was positively correlated with self-reports of alcohol craving.15 Left prefrontal16-19 and bilateral orbitofrontal cortices17,20-24 have commonly activated in response to substance cues among adults. Some studies have reported bilateral activation changes in the amygdala,16,17,21,22 but more have shown a left and bilateral anterior cingulate response that correlates with craving.16-18,20-22,25 The reward-related nucleus accumbens appears responsive to substance cues and is related to craving reports, bilaterally16,25 and on the left,26 although some studies could not precisely localize this small region. In the first functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study of pictoral alcohol cues, George and colleagues19 reported a thalamic and left prefrontal response to alcoholic relative to nonalcoholic beverage pictures (hereafter referred to as alcohol and nonalcohol pictures, respectively) among alcohol-dependent adults compared with matched control subjects. Our group previously studied brain responses of women aged 18 to 25 years to alcohol- and nonalcohol-related words.26 Alcohol-dependent young women demonstrated significantly more blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) response than controls during alcohol word presentation relative to neutral words in the anterior cingulate, left prefrontal cortex, bilateral insular gyri, and subcallosal cortex, which houses the nucleus accumbens. However, group differences were relatively modest.26

To summarize, a sizable literature reports that adults with substance use disorders demonstrate atypical reactions to stimuli that have been conditioned to addictive substances.27 However, the brain regions reported to subserve cue reactivity have been somewhat inconsistent, due in part to differing imaging techniques, stimuli, and populations, as previously reviewed.26 Cue-induced brain changes have not been studied in adolescents, although substance-dependent youths report substantial levels of craving,28 posttreatment relapses among adolescents are associated with exposure to substance-related cues,29 and youths report exposure to alcohol cues through advertising an average of 30 times per month.30 To assess the neural substrates of cue reactivity in youth, we studied adolescents aged 14 to 17 years, used pictures instead of words to more directly elicit cue reactivity, and used personally relevant alcohol stimuli. It was hypothesized that adolescents with alcohol use disorders (AUDs) would exhibit more brain activity in response to alcohol cues relative to teens without drinking problems, particularly in anterior cingulate, prefrontal, orbitofrontal, and subcallosal cortices. In addition, we hypothesized that desires to drink would correlate with levels of activation.

Methods
Participants

Recruitment flyers were distributed at local high schools. When a teen telephoned in response, a brief interview preliminarily ascertained eligibility, then the legal guardian was asked exclusionary questions. After a description of the study, written informed consent and assent, approved by the University of California–San Diego Institutional Review Board, were obtained from parents and adolescents. The teen was administered a 90-minute detailed screening interview, including the Family History Assessment Module screener,31 to assess family history of substance use and psychiatric diagnoses. We used the Customary Drinking and Drug Use Record32 to assess substance use and abuse/dependence criteria, and the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children to assess adolescent psychiatric diagnoses.33 The same measures were administered to the parent by a separate interviewer for corroboration. In cases of discrepancies, additional data were obtained or data were coded to represent the lower level of functioning or presence of problems.

Exclusionary criteria were a history of a DSM-IV psychiatric or substance disorder other than AUD, neurological illness, head trauma with loss of consciousness for longer than 2 minutes, serious medical problems and learning disability; current use of medications that could affect the central nervous system; smoking more than 4 cigarettes per day; significant maternal drinking during pregnancy (≥4 drinks per occasion or ≥7 drinks per week); family history of bipolar I or psychotic disorders; inadequate English skills; sensory problems; left-handedness; and irremovable metal on the body. Because of high comorbidity with substance use disorders,34,35 teens meeting criteria for conduct disorder (n = 2) were not excluded.

Participants with AUD (n = 15) met current DSM-IV criteria for alcohol abuse or dependence, and normal controls (n = 15) had very limited experience with alcohol or other drugs. Each group contained 6 girls and 9 boys with an average age of 16 years, mostly from upper middle-class families (Table 1). Based on neuropsychological tests administered the day of scanning, participants in both groups were above average intellectually. Youths with AUD typically drank 6 drinks each weekend night and had met criteria for alcohol abuse (n = 7) or dependence (n = 8) for 1 to 2 years. Youths with AUD reported higher levels of depressed mood and nervousness before scanning than controls, although all were in the reference range except 1 participant with AUD (Beck Depression Inventory score, 19; Spielberger Anxiety T score, 64). After excluding this participant, groups had similar Beck Depression Inventory scores. Subjects with AUD still showed more anxiety, although within the normal range (Spielberger Anxiety T score, 44).

Measures
Structured Clinical Interview

Teens and parents were administered a structured clinical interview36 by separate psychometrists covering demographic, medical, academic, family, and social functioning information. Parents were asked about the teen's developmental history and familial socioeconomic status37 and administered the Child Behavior Checklist.38

Substance Use and Diagnoses

Substance involvement and use disorder diagnoses were assessed with the Customary Drinking and Drug Use Record,32 which collects lifetime and past 3-month information on alcohol, nicotine, and other drug use and assesses DSM-IV abuse and dependence criteria,39,40 withdrawal symptoms, and other negative consequences of substance use. Strong psychometric properties have been demonstrated in adolescents.32,41 The Timeline Follow-back42 provided substance use patterns for the previous 30 days. All participants submitted urine samples for drug toxicologic screening.

Neuropsychological Testing

A 2-hour neuropsychological test battery was administered by a trained psychometrist (A.D.S.). The battery covered attention, working memory, learning and memory, and executive, visuospatial, and language functioning. General intellect was estimated with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III Vocabulary subtest,43 which correlates highly with full-scale IQ.43,44

State Measures

The Beck Depression Inventory45 and state scale of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory46 assessed mood at the time of scanning. The Stanford Sleepiness Scale measured alertness immediately before and after scanning with self-reported ratings (1 indicates alert; 7, almost asleep).47

Drinking Urge

Alcohol craving was evaluated immediately before and after scanning with a 100-mm visual analog scale for rating the urge to drink48 and the Desires for Alcohol Questionnaire (DAQ).49 The DAQ yields the following 3 factors: reinforcing effects, strong desires to drink, and mild desires to drink.

Procedures

Several days before scanning, participants were asked for their preferred alcoholic and nonalcoholic beverage brands and what brands they had consumed in the past year to guide individualized beverage picture selection. Participants were asked to abstain from alcohol and other drugs for at least 48 hours before imaging. The most recent drinking reported was 72 hours before scanning, and no withdrawal symptoms were reported or evident in any participant the day of scanning. All imaging sessions occurred on Thursdays from 8 to 10 PM to maximize recovery from weekend binge drinking and maintain consistent circadian influence across subjects. Once subjects arrived for the single assessment session, Breathalyzer (Intoximeter, Inc, St Louis, Mo) and urine samples were collected for drug toxicologic screening and, for girls, pregnancy screening. No participant had a measurable breath alcohol concentration, and only 1 participant, who had disclosed recent marijuana use (3 days before), had positive findings in a urine sample.

Teens underwent assessment by a trained bachelor-level psychometrist of the same sex (E.H.C. or A.D.S.). Throughout scheduling and the assessment session, adolescents were told about the imaging procedures and the importance of keeping as still as possible during scanning. This was boldasized again just before scanner entry by the MRI technologist. After lying in the scanner, a soft cloth was placed on the participant's forehead, which was then taped to the head coil to minimize head motion, and a response box was placed in the subject's right hand.

The scanning protocol consisted of high-resolution structural imaging (inversion-recovery-prepared, T1-weighted, sagittally acquired 3-dimensional spiral fast spin echo50; 16 interleaves; echo-train length, 8; repetition time, 2000 milliseconds; inversion time, 700 milliseconds; echo spacing, 15.6 milliseconds; echo time, 15.6 milliseconds; field of view, 240 mm; in-plane resolution, 0.9375 × 0.9375 mm; through-plane resolution, 1.328 mm; 128 continuous slices; acquisition time, 8 minutes 36 seconds) and axially-acquired T2*-weighted spiral gradient recall echo imaging (repetition time, 3000 milliseconds; echo time, 40 milliseconds; flip angle, 90°; field of view, 240 mm; 20-21 axial slices covering the whole brain; slice thickness, 7 mm; reconstructed in-plane resolution, 1.875 × 1.875 mm; 138 repetitions; acquisition time, 6 minutes 54 seconds). Spiral imaging was used because it helps reduce the effects of motion on time series acquisitions.51,52

During fMRI data collection, an alcohol pictures task was administered. This task sequentially presented 20 alcohol and 20 nonalcohol pictures matched by color, visual complexity, and presence of people. The images were selected from a bank of more than 200 advertisements scanned from youth-oriented popular magazines (eg, Rolling Stone, Spin, Sports Illustrated, and Cosmopolitan) or downloaded from the Internet. A personalized set of images was selected for each adolescent, on the basis of his or her alcoholic and nonalcoholic beverage preferences and experiences, to ensure familiarity with the stimuli. To maintain focus on the task without drawing attention to picture content, instructions consisted of the following: "Press 1 if the picture contains a person; press 2 if there is no person" (30% in each condition contained a person). The task alternated between 30-second blocks of each condition (10 trials per block; 2500-millisecond stimulus presentation and 500-second interstimulus interval), with fixation periods at the beginning, middle, and end (Figure 1 shows task design and stimulus samples).

Data analysis

Data were processed and analyzed with the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages package.53 First, we applied to the time series data a 3-dimensional motion-correction algorithm that aligned each volume in the time series to a selected base volume54 and estimated 3 rotational and 3 displacement parameters for each participant. To determine whether bulk motion differed between groups, each subject's absolute mean for each of the 6 motion parameters across the time series was compared in 1-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs). Controls required significantly more motion correction for 2 parameters (roll, 0.05 vs 0.03 mm [P = .02]; left displacement, 0.04 vs 0.02 mm [P =.05]) and showed more variability in motion during the time series for 2 parameters (roll SD, 0.05 vs 0.04 [P = .04]; yaw SD, 0.08 vs 0.05 [P = .02]), although the magnitude of these differences was quite small. To estimate task-correlated motion, the 6 parameters were correlated with the task reference vector across the time series for each subject. The median correlations for the AUD group were −0.069, 0.122, −0.011, −0.006, −0.067, and −0.037; for controls, −0.054, 0.059, 0.003, 0.042, −0.135, and −0.032 for roll, pitch, and yaw rotations and superior, left, and posterior displacements, respectively. Task-correlated values were compared between groups using Spearman correlations (P>.18 for all).

Next, the time series data were correlated with a set of 7 task reference vectors. These consisted of 1 seed reference function representing the alternating conditions during the time course of the task (depicted in Figure 1), and the same reference vector shifted in 1-second increments 6 times forward to account for delays in hemodynamic response,55 while covarying for linear trends and the estimated degree of motion (to control for spin history effects). Only the reference vector producing the highest correlation with the time course data was used, yielding fit coefficients for every subject in each voxel representing the correspondence between the observed and hypothesized signal. Each participant's imaging results were transformed into standard56 coordinates, and the functional map was resampled into isotropic voxels (3.5 mm3). We applied a spatial smoothing Gaussian filter (full width at half maximum, 3.5 mm) to manage individual variability in gyral structure. To test hypotheses, an independent-samples t test (α = .05, 2-tailed) compared groups on BOLD response contrast across the time series, essentially testing a group × condition interaction. Urge scores were correlated with BOLD response contrast for each voxel. Type I error was controlled by requiring that voxels surpass the specified α and compose clusters of more than 515 µL,57,58 yielding a voxelwise P of .00002 and a clusterwise P of .032.

Results

Response logging failed for 8 AUD group and 6 control participants. Using available data, groups did not differ between task conditions in reaction time (alcohol pictures, 805.76 vs 860.31 milliseconds; neutral pictures, 736.18 vs 840.83 milliseconds for AUD and control groups, respectively) or accuracy (alcohol pictures, 96% both groups; neutral pictures, 98% both groups). The AUD group reported significantly more desires to drink than controls for all 3 DAQ factors before and after scanning. However, no significant increases were found after scanning for either group on the DAQ or the visual analog scale scores. As a result, we averaged prescanning and postscanning DAQ scores for use in analyses.

Throughout the brain, the AUD group showed significantly more BOLD response than controls to alcohol pictures relative to neutral beverage pictures, particularly in the left hemisphere, including frontal and limbic regions (P<.05; effect sizes, 3.67-9.69). This included hypothesized regions (ie, the ventral anterior cingulate, prefrontal cortex, orbital gyrus, and subcallosal cortex) and other areas (eg, the inferior frontal gyrus, paracentral lobule, parahippocampus, amygdala, fusiform gyrus, temporal lobe, hypothalamus, posterior cingulate, precuneus, cuneus, and angular gyrus). In contrast, controls showed more BOLD response to alcohol pictures relative to neutral pictures than the AUD group in 2 right frontal regions (P<.05; Table 2 and Figure 2). This analysis was rerun excluding the AUD participant with higher levels of depressed mood and anxiety. All regions listed in Table 2 remained significant except 2, and the AUD group showed more response to alcohol pictures in the left inferior parietal lobule (Brodmann area 39) than controls. The group × condition interactions were confirmed using the 3-dimensional ANOVA2 program of the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages,53 comparing signals from alcohol and nonalcohol picture conditions relative to fixation for both groups. All regions listed in Table 2 were confirmed, except for the right inferior frontal gyrus and right lateral precuneus.

To understand the main effect of condition, we studied each group separately in single-sample t tests. The AUD group showed greater response to alcohol pictures relative to neutral pictures in 21 regions, whereas they showed increased response to neutral beverages in just 2 regions (P<.05). Controls had more response to alcoholic beverage pictures in 5 locations, yet more response to neutral pictures relative to alcoholic beverage pictures in 16 regions (P<.05). To examine the main effect of group, we contrasted the BOLD response between the alcoholic beverage pictures and fixation conditions. Both groups showed considerably more response to the alcoholic beverage pictures than to the fixation cross, but the AUD group showed a more extensive response contrast. However, this contrast and the contrast between neutral pictures and fixation revealed some BOLD response in the AUD group during fixation blocks, perhaps indicating continued reactivity to alcohol stimuli.

To test the relationship between self-reported desire to drink and brain response, the 3 DAQ factors (reinforcement, strong desires, and mild desires) were examined in regressions to predict the BOLD response contrast between the alcoholic and nonalcoholic beverage pictures for each group. Among those in the AUD group, the reinforcement factor did not predict variability in BOLD response contrast, but the strong desires factor predicted left temporal(β = 16.15) and right thalamic (β = −22.88) responses. The mild desires factor significantly and positively predicted 10 regions of enhanced BOLD response to alcoholic beverage pictures relative to neutral pictures in the AUD group (Table 3). Among controls, low reinforcement scores predicted more BOLD response contrast in the right posterior cingulate and temporal regions, and the strong and mild desires factors were unrelated to the BOLD response. To confirm these findings, we extracted each participant's signal intensity values from the regions that differed between groups (Table 2) and correlated these values with drinking and craving scores. For controls, only the following correlations were found: left temporal/fusiform BOLD signal contrast correlated with drinks consumed per month (r =0.56 [P = .03]), and left parahippocampal/amygdalar signal correlated with mild desires to drink (r =−0.55 [P = .03]). For the AUD group, 4 regions correlated positively with drinks per month (left inferior frontal, r = 0.51 [P = .04]; left paracentral lobule/dorsal cingulate, r = 0.59 [P = .02]; right precuneus/cuneus, r = 0.64[P = .01]; and right precuneus/posterior cingulate, r = 0.76 [P = .001]) (Figure 3), and 2 areas correlated negatively with reinforcement scores (right precuneus/cuneus, r =−0.73 [P = .002]; and right inferior frontal, r = −0.53 [P = .04]).

Because individuals with family histories of AUD tend to respond abnormally to alcohol and show other neural anomalies,59-62 we compared brain responses to alcohol pictures between those in the AUD group with family histories that were positive (FHP) and negative (FHN) for AUD. The AUD-group teens with FHP (n = 9; 5 [56%] female) showed more BOLD response contrast between the alcoholic and nonalcoholic beverage pictures than the AUD-group teens with FHN (n = 6; 1 [17%] female), especially in the left posterior cingulate and prefrontal, orbital, and inferior temporal gyrus. Controls with FHP (n = 9; 3 [33%] female) showed more brain response to alcoholic beverage pictures relative to nonalcoholic beverage pictures than controls with FHN(n = 6; 3 [50%] female), particularly in the left paracentral, medial frontal, prefrontal, cuneus, and anterior cingulate areas. However, when comparing the 9 AUD-group teens with FHP and the 9 controls with FHP, the AUD group showed substantially more response to alcoholic beverage pictures throughout the brain. Similarly, the 6 AUD-group teens with FHN showed more brain response to alcohol pictures than the 6 controls with FHN, except in the right frontal pole and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

In adolescence, sex differences in neuromaturation63-65 influence affective response66 and may relate to cue reactivity. We compared responses with alcohol pictures relative to nonalcohol pictures in boys and girls of the AUD and control groups. In the AUD group, girls (n = 6) showed more brain response than boys (n = 9) to alcohol pictures, particularly in the anterior cingulate and left prefrontal regions, whereas boys showed a strong response to alcohol pictures in the left orbital gyrus and bilateral paracentral gyri. No sex differences were apparent among controls.

Comment

Our results supported the hypothesis that adolescents with AUD produce more brain activity in response to alcohol cues than teens without drinking problems. This was specifically supported in the ventral anterior cingulate and subcallosal, prefrontal, orbital, and limbic regions, areas previously associated with reward and drug craving. In addition, we found increased response in posterior regions that may be critical to visual association, episodic recall, appetitive functions, and the formation of associations. In teens with alcohol abuse/dependence, mild desires to drink alcohol were associated with enhanced BOLD response in frontal and visual regions and diminished response in the ventral anterior cingulate.

These results are consistent with previous studies on alcohol and other drug cues. Like many neuroimaging studies of craving, we found an anterior cingulate response to substance cues among problem users.16-18,20-22,25,26,67,68 These findings also support studies that found subcallosal cortex/nucleus accumbens,16,25,26 orbitofrontal,17,20-24 left prefrontal,16-19,22,26,68 amygdala,17,21 temporal,17,23,67,69 and posterior cingulate16,22,23 responses. The most similar design to ours is the 2001 study by George and colleagues.19 Both studies found increased left prefrontal BOLD response in individuals with AUD during exposure to alcohol relative to nonalcohol pictures. Although our study did not replicate their thalamic activation, the hypothalamus of teens in our AUD group was more responsive to alcohol pictures than to nonalcohol pictures, whereas controls did not show such an effect.

The present study found larger areas of brain activation to alcohol pictures than the study by George et al.19 Several dissimilarities between projects could account for these differences. First, youth tend to exhibit larger areas of BOLD response relative to adults across tasks70,71 owing to regional specialization that develops throughout adolescence. Second, our study used personalized stimuli instead of a standard picture series. Many youths have used only certain alcoholic beverage types, and we wanted to ensure previous exposure to beverage pictures to maximize cue reactivity. Third, our study examined the whole brain, demonstrating group differences in posterior regions. Fourth, the current study repeated images, using 40 images, whereas George et al19 used 56 pictures. This leaves the possibility that recognition might have affected results. Fifth, our pulse sequences collected data spirally in k-space, a technique that is less sensitive to motion artifact.51,52 Finally, the larger sample size resulted in more statistical power to detect subtler effects.

These results confirm the findings of a recent study from our group26 in which alcohol-related cues appeared to provoke increased BOLD response in the nucleus accumbens region among college-age women with AUD. However, the magnitude of response in the present study is much larger and involved the visual system, possibly due to the pictoral stimulus modality and younger developmental stage of participants. The role of the visual system in the response of heavy drinkers to pictoral alcohol cues was not predicted, but it merits consideration. Feature-based visual attention may serve as the earliest stage of cortical response to visual stimuli and is linked to substantial BOLD response in visual brain regions.72 Because of personal experiences and affective responses, adolescents with AUD may have attended to a broader array of features in the alcohol images, involving more visual system neurons than were involved for controls. Future studies could examine this hypothesis using eye-movement measures. The response of the AUD group to alcohol pictures in the ventromedial region (Brodmann area 47) corresponds to findings of decision-making studies73,74 that showed the importance of this region for making selections based on reward contingencies.

Several limitations of the current study warrant consideration. First, teens did not report increased craving after the alcohol pictures task, whereas the adults in the study by George et al19 did, suggesting that dynamic craving processes were not captured in the imaging sessions. Teens tend to drink in the absence of adults, so the MRI setting may have been antithetical to typical drinking situations, and youths may be less able to discriminate and report changes in craving than adults. Urge ratings were not collected during scanning, but 5 minutes before and after scanner entry. Second, adolescents with AUD had more extensive histories of other drug and nicotine use than did controls, although sex, ethnicity, and family history were comparable. Third, our use of personalized cues may diminish generalizability and comparability between participants, and it is possible that social aspects of some pictures produced different responses across participants. Within-subject designs are needed to compare the effects of individualized vs standard cues.75 Fourth, although all participants had some familiarity with the stimuli presented, teens with AUD may have had more experience with the alcohol stimuli, so results may represent differential recall effects.

Conclusions

This fMRI study demonstrated that high school youths with alcohol abuse or dependence show widespread and intense brain activation in response to pictures of alcohol advertisements. These results suggest that, not only are liking and remembering alcohol advertisements associated with frequent drinking76 and expecting to drink more,77 but alcohol advertisements may have a strong effect on youths with established heavy drinking patterns as well as those with family histories of AUD, similar to how media depictions of aggression have detrimental effects on children with preexisting aggressive traits.78 For young drinkers, this neural response may indicate that advertisement content has been conditioned with drinking experiences, and may reflect increasing salience of alcohol advertisements as drinking escalates. Fortunately, encouraging youths to evaluate advertisements and critique the intentions of advertisers appears to help counter the negative influences.79,80 For teens in treatment for substance use disorders, research is needed to determine whether cue reactivity can be reduced and whether diminished response to cues predicts treatment success.

Corresponding author: Susan F. Tapert, PhD, Psychology Service (116B), Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System, 3350 La Jolla Village Dr, San Diego, CA 92161 (e-mail: stapert@ucsd.edu).

Submitted for publication August 23, 2002; final revision received February 6, 2003; accepted February 6, 2003.

This study was supported by grants R21 AA12519 and R01 AA13419 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, Bethesda, Md (Dr Tapert) and a Veterans Affairs Merit Grant (Dr Frank).

This study was presented in part at the annual meeting of the Research Society on Alcoholism, June 25, 2001; Montreal, Quebec.

We thank Lisa Eyler Zorrilla, PhD, Laura Santerre, Carmen Pulido, and Valerie Cestone for their contributions to this research.

References
1.
Tiffany ÌýSTÌýA cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes.ÌýÌýPsychol Rev. 1990;97147-Ìý168
2.
Tiffany ÌýSTConklin ÌýCAÌýA cognitive processing model of alcohol craving and compulsive alcohol use.ÌýÌý´¡»å»å¾±³¦³Ù¾±´Ç²Ô. 2000;95suppl 2S145-ÌýS153
3.
Anton ÌýRFDrobes ÌýDJÌýClinical measurement of craving in addiction.ÌýÌýPsychiatr Ann. 1998;28553-Ìý560
4.
McDonough ÌýBEWarren ÌýCAÌýEffects of 12-h tobacco deprivation on event-related potentials elicited by visual smoking cues.ÌýÌý±Ê²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²¹°ù³¾²¹³¦´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 2001;154282-Ìý291
5.
Monti ÌýPMRohsenow ÌýDJHutchison ÌýKEÌýToward bridging the gap between biological, psychobiological and psychosocial models of alcohol craving.ÌýÌý´¡»å»å¾±³¦³Ù¾±´Ç²Ô. 2000;95suppl 2S229-ÌýS236
6.
Monti ÌýPMBinkoff ÌýJAAbrams ÌýDBZwick ÌýWRNirenberg ÌýTDLiepman ÌýMRÌýReactivity of alcoholics and nonalcoholics to drinking cues.ÌýÌýJ Abnorm Psychol. 1987;96122-Ìý126
7.
Rohsenow ÌýDJMonti ÌýPMAbrams ÌýDBRubonis ÌýAVNiaura ÌýRSSirota ÌýADColby ÌýSMÌýCue elicited urge to drink and salivation in alcoholics: relationship to individual differences and relapse.ÌýÌýAdv Behav Res Ther. 1992;14195-Ìý210
8.
Stormark ÌýKMLaberg ÌýJCNordby ÌýHHugdahl ÌýKÌýAlcoholics' selective attention to alcohol stimuli: automated processing?ÌýÌýJ Stud Alcohol. 2000;6118-Ìý23
9.
McCusker ÌýCGBrown ÌýKÌýCue-exposure to alcohol-associated stimuli reduces autonomic reactivity, but not craving and anxiety, in dependent drinkers.ÌýÌýAlcohol Alcohol. 1995;30319-Ìý327
10.
Stormark ÌýKMField ÌýNPHugdahl ÌýKHorowitz ÌýMÌýSelective processing of visual alcohol cues in abstinent alcoholics: an approach-avoidance conflict?ÌýÌýAddict Behav. 1997;22509-Ìý519
11.
Johnsen ÌýBHLaberg ÌýJCCox ÌýWMVaksdal ÌýAHugdahl ÌýKÌýAlcoholic subjects' attentional bias in the processing of alcohol-related words.ÌýÌýPsychol Addict Behav. 1994;8111-Ìý115
12.
Stetter ÌýFAckermann ÌýKBizer ÌýAStraube ÌýERMann ÌýKÌýEffects of disease-related cues in alcoholic inpatients: results of a controlled "alcohol Stroop" study.ÌýÌýAlcohol Clin Exp Res. 1995;19593-Ìý599
13.
Myers ÌýMGBrown ÌýSAMott ÌýMAÌýCoping as a predictor of adolescent substance abuse treatment outcome.ÌýÌýJ Subst Abuse. 1993;515-Ìý29
14.
Hommer ÌýDWÌýFunctional imaging of craving.ÌýÌýAlcohol Res Health. 1999;23187-Ìý196
15.
Modell ÌýJGMountz ÌýJMÌýFocal cerebral blood flow change during craving for alcohol measured by SPECT.ÌýÌýJ Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1995;715-Ìý22
16.
Breiter ÌýHCGollub ÌýRLWeisskoff ÌýRMKennedy ÌýDNMakris ÌýNBerke ÌýJDGoodman ÌýJMKantor ÌýHLGastfriend ÌýDRRiorden ÌýJPMathew ÌýRTRosen ÌýBRHyman ÌýSEÌýAcute effects of cocaine on human brain activity and emotion.ÌýÌý±·±ð³Ü°ù´Ç²Ô. 1997;19591-Ìý611
17.
Grant ÌýSLondon ÌýEDNewlin ÌýDBVillemagne ÌýVLLiu ÌýXContoreggi ÌýCPhillips ÌýRLKimes ÌýASMargolin ÌýAÌýActivation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving.ÌýÌýProc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;9312040-Ìý12045
18.
Maas ÌýLCLukas ÌýSEKaufman ÌýMJWeiss ÌýRDDaniels ÌýSLRogers ÌýVWKukes ÌýTJRenshaw ÌýPFÌýFunctional magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activation during cue-induced cocaine craving.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1998;155124-Ìý126
19.
George ÌýMSAnton ÌýRFBloomer ÌýCTeneback ÌýCDrobes ÌýDJLorberbaum ÌýJPNahas ÌýZVincent ÌýDJÌýActivation of prefrontal cortex and anterior thalamus in alcoholic subjects on exposure to alcohol-specific cues.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58345-Ìý352
20.
Volkow ÌýNDWang ÌýG-JFowler ÌýJSHitzemann ÌýRAngrist ÌýBGatley ÌýSJLogan ÌýJDing ÌýY-SPappas ÌýNÌýAssociation of methylphenidate-induced craving with changes in right striato-orbitofrontal metabolism in cocaine abusers: implications in addiction.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1999;15619-Ìý26
21.
Childress ÌýARMozley ÌýPDMcElgin ÌýWFitzgerald ÌýJReivich ÌýMO'Brien ÌýCPÌýLimbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1999;15611-Ìý18Google Scholar
22.
Garavan ÌýHPankiewicz ÌýJBloom ÌýACho ÌýJKSperry ÌýLRoss ÌýTJSalmeron ÌýBJRisinger ÌýRKelley ÌýDStein ÌýEAÌýCue-induced cocaine craving: neuroanatomical specificity for drug users and drug stimuli.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 2000;1571789-Ìý1798
23.
Sell ÌýLAMorris ÌýJSBearn ÌýJFrackowiak ÌýRSFriston ÌýKJDolan ÌýRJÌýNeural responses associated with cue evoked emotional states and heroin in opiate addicts.ÌýÌýDrug Alcohol Depend. 2000;60207-Ìý216
24.
Wang ÌýGJVolkow ÌýNDFowler ÌýJSCervany ÌýPHitzemann ÌýRJPappas ÌýNRWong ÌýCTFelder ÌýCÌýRegional brain metabolic activation during craving elicited by recall of previous drug experiences.ÌýÌýLife Sci. 1999;64775-Ìý784
25.
Kilts ÌýCDSchweitzer ÌýJBQuinn ÌýCKGross ÌýREFaber ÌýTLMuhammad ÌýFEly ÌýTDHoffman ÌýJMDrexler ÌýKPÌýNeural activity related to drug craving in cocaine addiction.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58334-Ìý341
26.
Tapert ÌýSFBrown ÌýGGBaratta ÌýMVBrown ÌýSAÌýfMRI BOLD response to alcohol stimuli in alcohol dependent young women.ÌýÌýAddict Behav. In press
27.
Weinstein ÌýAFeldtkeller ÌýBMalizia ÌýAWilson ÌýSBailey ÌýJNutt ÌýDJÌýIntegrating the cognitive and physiological aspects of craving.ÌýÌýJ Psychopharmacol. 1998;1231-Ìý38
28.
Martin ÌýCKaczynski ÌýNMaisto ÌýSBukstein ÌýOMoss ÌýHÌýPatterns of DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence symptoms in adolescent drinkers.ÌýÌýJ Stud Alcohol. 1995;56672-Ìý680
29.
Brown ÌýSATapert ÌýSFTate ÌýSRAbrantes ÌýAMÌýThe role of alcohol in adolescent relapse and outcome.ÌýÌýJ Psychoactive Drugs. 2000;32107-Ìý115
30.
Martin ÌýSESnyder ÌýLBHamilton ÌýMFleming-Milici ÌýFSlater ÌýMDStacy ÌýAChen ÌýMJGrube ÌýJWÌýAlcohol advertising and youth.ÌýÌýAlcohol Clin Exp Res. 2002;26900-Ìý906
31.
Rice ÌýJPReich ÌýTBucholz ÌýKKNeuman ÌýRJFishman ÌýRRochberg ÌýNHesselbrock ÌýVMNurnberger ÌýJI ÌýJrSchuckit ÌýMABegleiter ÌýHÌýComparison of direct interview and family history diagnoses of alcohol dependence.ÌýÌýAlcohol Clin Exp Res. 1995;191018-Ìý1023
32.
Brown ÌýSAMyers ÌýMGLippke ÌýLTapert ÌýSFStewart ÌýDGVik ÌýPWÌýPsychometric evaluation of the Customary Drinking and Drug Use Record (CDDR): a measure of adolescent alcohol and drug involvement.ÌýÌýJ Stud Alcohol. 1998;59427-Ìý438
33.
Shaffer ÌýDFisher ÌýPLucas ÌýCPDulcan ÌýMKSchwab-Stone ÌýMEÌýNIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses.ÌýÌýJ Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;3928-Ìý38
34.
Brown ÌýSAGleghorn ÌýAASchuckit ÌýMAMyers ÌýMGMott ÌýMAÌýConduct disorder among adolescent alcohol and drug abusers.ÌýÌýJ Stud Alcohol. 1996;57314-Ìý324
35.
Myers ÌýMGStewart ÌýDGBrown ÌýSAÌýProgression from conduct disorder to antisocial personality disorder following treatment for adolescent substance abuse.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1998;155479-Ìý485
36.
Brown ÌýSAVik ÌýPWCreamer ÌýVAÌýCharacteristics of relapse following adolescent substance abuse treatment.ÌýÌýAddict Behav. 1989;14291-Ìý300
37.
Hollingshead ÌýABÌýTwo-Factor Index of Social Position.Ìý New Haven, Conn Yale University Press1965;
38.
Achenbach ÌýTMÌýManual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile.Ìý Burlington University of Vermont Dept of Psychiatry1991;
39.
American Psychiatric Association,ÌýDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition.Ìý Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1987;
40.
American Psychiatric Association,ÌýDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition.Ìý Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
41.
Stewart ÌýDGBrown ÌýSAÌýWithdrawal and dependency symptoms among adolescent alcohol and drug abusers.ÌýÌý´¡»å»å¾±³¦³Ù¾±´Ç²Ô. 1995;90627-Ìý635
42.
Sobell ÌýLCSobell ÌýMBÌýTimeline Follow-back: a technique for assessing self-reported alcohol consumption.ÌýRaye ÌýZLitten ÌýJPAedÌýMeasuring Alcohol Consumption: Psychosocial and Biochemical Methods. Totowa, NJ Humana Press Inc1992;41-Ìý72
43.
Wechsler ÌýDÌýManual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children III.Ìý San Antonio, Tex Psychological Corp1993;
44.
Sattler ÌýJMÌýAssessment of Children, Third Edition Revised, Updated.Ìý San Diego, Calif JM Sattler1992;
45.
Beck ÌýATÌýBeck Depression Inventory (BDI).Ìý San Antonio, Tex Psychological Corp1978;
46.
Spielberger ÌýCDGorsuch ÌýRLLushene ÌýREÌýManual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.Ìý Palo Alto, Calif Consulting Psychologists Press1970;
47.
Glenville ÌýMBroughton ÌýRÌýReliability of the Stanford Sleepiness Scale compared to short duration performance tests and the Wilkinson Auditory Vigilance Task.ÌýÌýAdv Biosci. 1978;21235-Ìý244
48.
Chick ÌýJHowlett ÌýHMorgan ÌýMYRitson ÌýBÌýUnited Kingdom Multicentre Acamprosate Study (UKMAS): a 6-month prospective study of acamprosate versus placebo in preventing relapse after withdrawal from alcohol.ÌýÌýAlcohol Alcohol. 2000;35176-Ìý187
49.
Love ÌýAJames ÌýDWillner ÌýPÌýA comparison of two alcohol craving questionnaires.ÌýÌý´¡»å»å¾±³¦³Ù¾±´Ç²Ô. 1998;931091-Ìý1102
50.
Wong ÌýECLuh ÌýWMBuxton ÌýRBFrank ÌýLRÌýSingle slab high resolution 3D whole brain imaging using spiral FSE.ÌýÌýProc Intl Soc Magn Reson Med. 2000;8683
51.
Meyer ÌýCHHu ÌýBSNishimura ÌýDGMacovski ÌýAÌýFast spiral coronary artery imaging.ÌýÌýMagn Reson Med. 1992;28202-Ìý213
52.
Noll ÌýDCPauly ÌýJMMeyer ÌýCHNishimura ÌýDGMacovski ÌýAÌýDeblurring for non-2D Fourier transform magnetic resonance imaging.ÌýÌýMagn Reson Med. 1992;25319-Ìý333
53.
Cox ÌýRWÌýAFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages.ÌýÌýComput Biomed Res. 1996;29162-Ìý173
54.
Cox ÌýRWJesmanowicz ÌýAÌýReal-time 3D image registration for functional MRI.ÌýÌýMagn Reson Med. 1999;421014-Ìý1018
55.
Bandettini ÌýPAJesmanowicz ÌýAWong ÌýECHyde ÌýJSÌýProcessing strategies for time-course data sets in functional MRI of the human brain.ÌýÌýMagn Reson Med. 1993;30161-Ìý173
56.
Talairach ÌýJTournoux ÌýPÌýCoplanar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: Three-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging.Ìý New York, NY Thieme-Stratton Inc1988;
57.
Forman ÌýSDCohen ÌýJDFitzgerald ÌýMEddy ÌýWFMintun ÌýMANoll ÌýDCÌýImproved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold.ÌýÌýMagn Reson Med. 1995;33636-Ìý647
58.
Ward ÌýBDÌýSimultaneous Inference for fMRI Data.Ìý Milwaukee, Wis Biophysics Research Institute, Medical College ofWisconsin1997;
59.
Schuckit ÌýMAGold ÌýEOCroot ÌýKFinn ÌýPPolich ÌýJÌýP300 latency after ethanol ingestion in sons of alcoholics and in controls.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1988;24310-Ìý315
60.
Schuckit ÌýMASmith ÌýTLÌýThe relationships of a family history of alcohol dependence, a low level of response to alcohol and six domains of life functioning to the development of alcohol use disorders.ÌýÌýJ Stud Alcohol. 2000;61827-Ìý835
61.
Ehlers ÌýCLSchuckit ÌýMAÌýEvaluation of EEG alpha activity in sons of alcoholics.ÌýÌýNeuropsychopharmacology. 1991;4199-Ìý205
62.
Hada ÌýMPorjesz ÌýBChorlian ÌýDBBegleiter ÌýHPolich ÌýJÌýAuditory P3a deficits in male subjects at high risk for alcoholism.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 2001;49726-Ìý738
63.
Giedd ÌýJNVaituzis ÌýACHamburger ÌýSDLange ÌýNRajapakse ÌýJCKaysen ÌýDVauss ÌýYCRapoport ÌýJLÌýQuantitative MRI of the temporal lobe, amygdala, and hippocampus in normal human development: ages 4-18 years.ÌýÌýJ Comp Neurol. 1996;366223-Ìý230
64.
Schlaepfer ÌýTEHarris ÌýGJTien ÌýAYPeng ÌýLLee ÌýSPearlson ÌýGDÌýStructural differences in the cerebral cortex of healthy female and male subjects: a magnetic resonance imaging study.ÌýÌýPsychiatry Res. 1995;61129-Ìý135
65.
Caviness ÌýVS ÌýJrKennedy ÌýDNRichelme ÌýCRademacher ÌýJFilipek ÌýPAÌýThe human brain age 7-11 years: a volumetric analysis based on magnetic resonance images.ÌýÌýCereb Cortex. 1996;6726-Ìý736
66.
Killgore ÌýWDOki ÌýMYurgelun-Todd ÌýDAÌýSex-specific developmental changes in amygdala responses to affective faces.ÌýÌýNeuroreport. 2001;12427-Ìý433
67.
Wexler ÌýBEGottschalk ÌýCHFulbright ÌýRKProhovnik ÌýILacadie ÌýCMRounsaville ÌýBJGore ÌýJCÌýFunctional magnetic resonance imaging of cocaine craving.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 2001;15886-Ìý95
68.
Daglish ÌýMRCWeinstein ÌýAMalazia ÌýALWilson ÌýSMelichar ÌýJKBritten ÌýSBrewer ÌýCLingford-Hughes ÌýAMyles ÌýJSGrasby ÌýPNutt ÌýDJÌýChanges in regional cerebral blood flow elicited by craving memories in abstinent opiate-dependent subjects.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 2001;1581680-Ìý1686
69.
Schneider ÌýFHabel ÌýUWagner ÌýMFranke ÌýPSalloum ÌýJBShah ÌýNJToni ÌýISulzbach ÌýCHonig ÌýKMaier ÌýWGaebel ÌýWZilles ÌýKÌýSubcortical correlates of craving in recently abstinent alcoholic patients.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 2001;1581075-Ìý1083
70.
Gaillard ÌýWDHertz-Pannier ÌýLMott ÌýSHBarnett ÌýASLeBihan ÌýDTheodore ÌýWHÌýFunctional anatomy of cognitive development: fMRI of verbal fluency in children and adults.ÌýÌýNeurology. 2000;54180-Ìý185
71.
Thomas ÌýKMKing ÌýSWFranzen ÌýPLWelsh ÌýTFBerkowitz ÌýALNoll ÌýDCBirmaher ÌýVCasey ÌýBJÌýA developmental functional MRI study of spatial working memory.ÌýÌýNeuroimage. 1999;10pt 1327-Ìý338
72.
Saenz ÌýMBuracas ÌýGTBoynton ÌýGMÌýGlobal effects of feature-based attention in human visual cortex.ÌýÌýNat Neurosci. 2002;5631-Ìý632
73.
Bechara ÌýADamasio ÌýHÌýDecision-making and addiction, I: impaired activation of somatic states in substance dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences.ÌýÌýNeuropsychologia. 2002;401675-Ìý1689
74.
Elliott ÌýRRees ÌýGDolan ÌýRJÌýVentromedial prefrontal cortex mediates guessing.ÌýÌýNeuropsychologia. 1999;37403-Ìý411
75.
Gawin ÌýFHÌýThe scientific exegesis of desire: neuroimaging crack craving.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58342-Ìý344
76.
Casswell ÌýSZhang ÌýJ-FSilva ÌýPAÌýAlcohol in the mass media and drinking by adolescents: a longitudinal study.ÌýÌý´¡»å»å¾±³¦³Ù¾±´Ç²Ô. 1994;891255-Ìý1263
77.
Wyllie ÌýAZhang ÌýJFCasswell ÌýSÌýResponses to televised alcohol advertisements associated with drinking behaviour of 10-17-year-olds.ÌýÌý´¡»å»å¾±³¦³Ù¾±´Ç²Ô. 1998;93361-Ìý371
78.
Johnson ÌýJGCohen ÌýPSmailes ÌýEMKasen ÌýSBrook ÌýJSÌýTelevision viewing and aggressive behavior during adolescence and adulthood.ÌýÌýScience. 2002;2952468-Ìý2471
79.
Slater ÌýMDRouner ÌýDMurphy ÌýKBeauvais ÌýFVan Leuven ÌýJDomenech-Rodriguez ÌýMMÌýAdolescent counterarguing of TV beer advertisements: evidence for effectiveness of alcohol education and critical viewing discussions.ÌýÌýJ Drug Educ. 1996;26143-Ìý158
80.
Agostinelli ÌýGGrube ÌýJWÌýAlcohol counter-advertising and the media: a review of recent research.ÌýÌýAlcohol Res Health. 2002;2615-Ìý21
×