BackgroundÌý
Certain aspects of the personality may be associated with the vulnerability to develop depression. A sib-pair method has been used to examine the familiality of the 7 scales of the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) and whether this could be related to the genetic vulnerability to develop depression.
MethodsÌý
Probands with depression and their nearest-aged siblings from Wales were compared with healthy control probands and their nearest-aged siblings on the TCI and measures of depressed mood.
ResultsÌý
All 7 scales of the TCI were familial, and scores on 6 of the scales were similar to US population scores. However, the Welsh subjects' scores on the self-transcendence scale were markedly lower than the US mean, suggesting strong cultural or national influences on this measure. Harm avoidance scores were substantially influenced by current and past depression, but this scale also showed stable traitlike characteristics that are likely related to the genetic vulnerability to depression. Novelty seeking and self-directedness were also partly state-dependent and were negatively correlated with low mood; high scorers may be resilient to the development of depression. High reward dependence may also protect against the development of depression and is unrelated to mood state. The cooperativeness, persistence, and self-transcendence scales appear to have a limited relationship with the development of depression.
ConclusionsÌý
Harm avoidance, reward dependence, novelty seeking, and self-directedness have traitlike characteristics that are related to the familiality of depression. Cooperativeness, self-transcendence, and persistence are also familial, but this appears to be unrelated to depression.
IN THEIR psychobiological model of the structure and development of personality, Cloninger et al1 describe 4 dimensions of temperament that are "independently heritable, manifest early in life, and involve preconceptual biases in perceptual memory and habit formation."1(p975) These dimensions are harm avoidance, novelty seeking, persistence, and reward dependence. The authors also describe 3 character dimensions: self-directedness, cooperativeness, and self-transcendence. These items are related to "acceptance of the individual self, acceptance of other people and of nature in general."1(p979)
Although the differences between individuals according to the temperament dimensions are observable in early childhood and are moderately predictive of adult behavior, character dimensions are defined in terms of "insight learning or reorganization of self-concepts."1(p975) Such self-concepts involve the extent to which the individual identifies the self as an autonomous individual (self-directedness), an integral part of society (cooperativeness), or an integral part of the universe as a whole (self-transcendence). The Temperament and Character Inventory1 (TCI) is a 226-item self-report, true-false questionnaire with 107 questions relating to the 4 temperament dimensions and 119 items for the 3 character dimensions. Scores are summed to provide quantitative scales for each dimension.
The relationship between TCI traits and mood disorders has been examined by several authors.2-4 Sato et al2 showed that the factor structure of the TCI was not disrupted by mild to moderate severity of depression and that this instrument had promise for characterizing the underlying personality structure in depression. Kusunoki et al3 showed that patients with depression had higher harm avoidance and lower cooperativeness and self-directedness scores than controls. The TCI traits have also been examined during the course of recovery from depression,4 when it was noted that harm avoidance scores remained elevated following recovery of mood.
One of the recurrent problems with studies that examine which aspects of personality are associated with vulnerability to depression is that measures of personality traits may be contaminated by mood state, and the 2 are often difficult to separate.5 We used a sib-pair design in the Cardiff Depression Study6,7 to tackle this problem. The design facilitates the investigation of both the relationship between TCI traits and depression and the familiality of the 7 scales and whether this is related to the genetic vulnerability to develop depression. This article discusses the relationship between the TCI and various depression measures as well as the familiality of the scales.
The methods of subject selection and evaluation have been described elsewhere6,7 and were as follows.
Proband and sibling recruitment
One hundred eight probands with depression (D-probands) were recruited following weekly medical record reviews by 2 psychiatrists (S.S. and A.M.) of admissions to psychiatric facilities (inpatient wards, outpatient clinics, day hospitals, or referrals to community mental health teams) based in the neighboring Welsh counties of Gwent and South Glamorgan and in the adjacent city of Cardiff. They were also recruited from among those receiving repeated prescriptions for antidepressant medication from 2 family physicians in Cardiff. The patients were sent letters describing the study via their family physician (FP) and asked if they would be interested in participation. Those who replied positively were then contacted directly to provide further details of the study and determine whether they fulfilled the following inclusion criteria: All D-probands were aged 18 to 65 years, fulfilled DSM-IV8 operational criteria for major depressive disorder, single or recurrent episode (section 296.2 or 296.3), and had a sibling (D-sib) willing to participate in the study. Subjects with a lifetime-ever history of psychotic or bipolar symptoms were excluded from further study, as were those for whom it was not possible to recruit their sibling. Thirty-seven D-probands were recruited from inpatient units, 48 from ambulatory services, and 23 from FP services.
Age- and sex-matched control subjects (C-probands) were recruited from dental and orthopedic outpatient clinics and from among the employees of the University Hospital of Wales National Health Service Trust (Cardiff). They were recruited if they had no history of depression and a sibling (C-sib) who was willing to participate in the study. For both D-probands and C-probands, the sibling nearest in age was recruited whenever possible. However, if this sibling was unavailable or unwilling, the sibling next closest in age was asked to participate. Although most interviews were conducted face-to-face,18.5% of the D-sibs and 33.3% of the C-sibs were interviewed by telephone. The study received approval from the South Glamorgan and Gwent Research Ethics Committees, and all subjects gave written informed consent before participation.
Initially, 119 D-probands and D-sibs and 148 C-probands and C-sibs were recruited. Information was incomplete for one or both members of 11 pairs in the depression group and 22 pairs in the control group. Complete information was obtained for 108 D-proband (38 men and 70 women) and D-sib (50 men and 75 women) pairs and 126 C-proband (44 men and 82 women) and C-sib (50 men and 76 women) pairs. Because there was a significant age difference between the D-probands and C-probands, which could potentially confound the findings, the 21 youngest control pairs were omitted from the data set. The final analyses were carried out on 108 D-probands and D-sibs and 105 C-probands and C-sibs(C-probands: 27 men and 78 women; C-sibs: 42 men and 63 women).
Interviews and self-rating questionnaires
All subjects were interviewed using the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry 9 They also completed several self-report questionnaires including the TCI1 and Beck Depression Inventory10 (BDI). Information obtained at the SCAN interview was entered into the interview's computerized scoring program (CATEGO5) to obtain DSM-IV diagnoses. SPSS statistical software (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Ill), version 8 for Windows, was used to create a database and undertake the statistical analyses.
Demographic details of the subject groups
The subjects have been described in greater detail elsewhere.6,7 The percentage of men and the mean age for each subject group were as follows: 108 D-probands: 35% men; mean ± SEM age, 39.8 ± 1.0 years; 108 D-sibs: 31% men; mean ± SEM age, 38.6 ± 1.0 years; 105 C-probands: 26% men; mean ± SEM age, 36.2 ± 1.2 years; 105 C-sibs: 40% men; mean ± SEM age, 39.1 ± 1.2 years. There were no significant differences between groups for age or sex and no significant differences between the D-probands and C-probands for marital status. Only 52% of the D-probands were in paid employment compared with 82% of C-probands because an excess of D-probands were registered as disabled from their illness.6 There were also no differences in scores for any of the scales of the TCI or BDI according to social class when subjects' occupations were broadly divided into "white collar" vs "blue collar."
Of the D-probands, 37 were recruited from inpatient units (IP) and had significantly higher BDI scores than those from the other 2 recruitment sources, ambulatory services (OP) and FPs (IP: mean ± SD BDI score, 35.81 ± 15.30; OP: mean ± SD BDI score, 27.33 ± 12.35; FP: mean ± SD BDI score, 25.61 ± 13.09; analysis of variance, F2,105 =5.51; P = .005). In addition, D-probands recruited from FPs were significantly younger than those recruited via mental health services (IP and OP) (IP: mean ± SD age, 41.7 ± 10.4 years; OP: mean ± SD age, 41.4 ± 10.6 years; FP: mean ± SD age, 33.3 ± 9.2 years; analysis of variance, F2,105 = 5.91; P = .004). However, there were no differences between D-probands recruited from the 3 different sources for any of the 7 scales of the TCI.
Most C-probands were recruited after responding to the request for volunteers from employees of the University Hospital of Wales National Health Service Trust. Only 24 (23%) were from outpatient clinics. Only 65% of D-probands were able to recruit their nearest-aged sibling compared with 75% of C-probands(χ21 = 5.33; P = .02).
Fifty-five siblings (19 D-sibs and 35 C-sibs) were interviewed by telephone, whereas the remaining 154 were interviewed face-to-face. Although there were no significant differences between siblings for any of the TCI scales, they did occur between groups for BDI score (face-to-face interview: mean ± SEM, 7.04 ± 0.67; telephone interview: mean ± SEM, 4.18 ± 0.57; t181.95 = 3.24; P = .001). However, when the 8 D-sibs with depression were removed from the analyses, there were no significant differences for BDI score. There were no significant differences between siblings for sex or analysis of variance with BDI as the outcome variable, and there was no interaction between type of interview and sex.
Scores on the cooperativeness and self-directedness scales were significantly positively correlated with age (cooperativeness: Pearson r = 0.16; P = .002; self-directedness: Pearson r = 0.15; P = .002). Novelty seeking was significantly negatively correlated with age (Pearson r = −0.32; P<.001).
Mean ± SEM scores for 135 men and 270 women are listed in Table 1, which also provides published means and SEs from a US population sample1 for comparison. Table 1 shows that cooperativeness, harm avoidance, reward dependence, and self-transcendence were significantly higher in women than men. For cooperativeness, the mean ± SEM score was 34.38 ± 0.34 for women and 31.94 ± 0.54 for men (t239.08 = −3.83; P<.001; 2-tailed test). The mean ± SEM score for harm avoidance was 17.70 ± 0.56 for women and 13.65 ± 0.77 for men (t271.78 = −4.26; P<.001; 2-tailed test). The mean ± SEM score for reward dependence was 16.90 ± 0.24 for women and 13.99 ± 0.35 for men (t260.09 = −6.94; P<.001; 2-tailed test). For self-transcendence, the mean ± SEM score was 12.86 ± 0.36 for women and 11.33 ± 0.52 for men (t264.94 = −2.39; P = .02; 2-tailed test).
The mean scores for the Welsh subjects were similar to the published means for the US population except for persistence, in which those from Wales scored slightly lower, and self-transcendence, for which the Welsh subjects (including the healthy controls) had markedly lower mean scores. For harm avoidance, the total mean scores for men and women appeared higher than the US means. As Table 2 indicates, this is because the Welsh group had a large proportion of subjects with depression who had elevated harm avoidance scores. The mean scores for C-probands and C-sibs without depression, listed in Table 2, were similar to those for the US population.
Tci scales and depression ratings at time of interview
All D-probands fulfilled the DSM-IV criteria for major depressive disorder. Thirty-six (33.3%) of the D-probands were experiencing their first episode of depression, whereas the remaining 72 (66.7%) had also been depressed in the past. Nineteen of the D-sibs reported a past episode of depression causing impairment, and all of these subjects had received treatment for their depression. Nine were depressed at the time of the interview (with 7 subjects reporting this as a first episode). Five of the C-sibs reported previous treated episodes of depression, but none were depressed at the time of the interview. The relative risk for past or current episodes of treated depression in D-sibs compared with C-sibs was 5.42.6
Mean scores and SEMs for the 7 TCI scales in the 4 groups of participants are listed in Table 2. Analysis of variance showed significant differences between the 4 groups for harm avoidance(F3,401 = 87.76; P<.001), cooperativeness(F3,401 = 6.45; P<.001), novelty seeking(F3,401 = 7.47; P<.001), and self-directedness(F3,401 = 52.15; P<.001). Post hoc testing using the Tukey test indicated significant differences between the D-probands plus D-sibs and C-probands plus C-sibs for cooperativeness and novelty seeking and between D-probands and the other 3 groups for harm avoidance and self-directedness. In addition, the harm avoidance and self-directedness scores of the D-sibs were intermediate between the D-probands and both control groups, and these scores were also significantly different in the D-sibs compared with the C-probands and C-sibs.
Table 3 indicates the correlations between the 7 scales of the TCI and the BDI scores for all 4 groups combined. Scores on the harm avoidance (Pearson r = 0.67; P<.001) and self-transcendence (Pearson r = 0.12; P = .01) scales are positively correlated with BDI scores. Scores on the cooperativeness (Pearson r = −0.23; P<.001), novelty seeking (Pearson r = −0.18; P<.001), and self-directedness (Pearson r = −0.65; P<.001) scales are significantly negatively correlated with BDI scores.
Familiality of the 7 tci scales
Table 3 also indicates the correlations for sib-pairs with depression and controls combined. All 7 scales are significantly correlated across sib-pairs, with the correlation coefficients ranging from 0.14 for reward dependence to 0.28 for harm avoidance and self-directedness.
Tci scales and history of depression
Nineteen of the D-sibs had had a past episode of depression but were not ill at the time of the interview, whereas 80 D-sibs had never been depressed. Mean scores and SEMs for the 7 TCI scales in these 2 groups of D-sibs are listed in Table 4. The D-sibs who had previously been depressed had significantly higher harm avoidance scores compared with D-sibs with no history of depression (t24.41 = −2.42; P = .02; 2-tailed test). The D-sibs who had never been depressed had significantly higher reward dependence scores compared with D-sibs with a history of depression(t28.99 = 2.59; P =.02; 2-tailed test). For the remaining scales, there were no significant differences between D-sibs with or without a history of depression.
Tci scale scores in never-depressed d-sibs and c-sibs
As mentioned, 80 D-sibs reported never having been depressed. These subjects were compared with 100 never-depressed C-sibs on the 7 scales of the TCI; mean scores and SEMs appear in Table 4. Never-depressed D-sibs had significantly higher harm avoidance scores (t161.11 = 2.36; P = .02; 2-tailed test) and significantly lower self-directedness scores(t121.69 = −2.38; P = .02; 2-tailed test) than never-depressed C-sibs. Novelty seeking and reward dependence were also significantly lower in never-depressed D-sibs compared with never-depressed C-sibs (novelty seeking: t173.60 = −1.77; P = .04; 1-tailed test; reward dependence: t154.54 =1.74; P = .04; 1-tailed test).
The results show that the D-probands who were inpatients when they participated in the study had higher scores on the BDI when compared with the OP and FP groups. This is not surprising; those who require admission to the hospital are likely to have more severe illnesses than those who remain in ambulatory facilities or with their FP for treatment. We have also shown that D-probands recruited from family practices were significantly younger than those recruited via OP or IP sources. Again, this is not surprising because such patients are probably nearer to the onset of their illness course. Nevertheless, the source of recruitment of the probands did not influence the scores on our main measures of interest in this article, the TCI scales. Furthermore, all D-probands fulfilled the same diagnostic criteria.
As we have shown elsewhere,6 this disorder was substantially familial, with 19 D-sibs reporting past treatment for depression and 8 more who were depressed at the time of the interview. This can be compared with 5 of the C-sibs who reported a history of depression but none of whom were depressed at the time of the interview. The SCAN interview consisted of a comprehensive clinical inquiry about the duration and severity of individual items regarding depressive psychopathlogic characteristics. A computerized scoring program was then used to apply the DSM-IV diagnoses. However, the interview did not evaluate subclinical or subthreshold symptoms in the subjects, nor were other comorbid disorders evaluated in detail with the exception of the exclusion diagnoses (psychotic symptoms or bipolar disorder).
Age, sex, and tci scale scores
We found significant sex differences for harm avoidance, reward dependence, cooperativeness, and self-transcendence; women had significantly higher scores than men. These traits are generally considered to be female rather than male characteristics, and similar findings have been noted in previous studies. Cloninger et al1 reported higher scores for cooperativeness and the "spiritual acceptance" aspect of the self-transcendence scale in women in the general population, whereas Mendlowicz et al11 found that healthy women had significantly higher reward dependence scores than men. Cloninger et al1 also reported positive correlations between age and self-directedness and cooperativeness, as found in the Cardiff Depression Study.
Mean tci scale scores in us and welsh subjects
Cloninger et al1 have reported mean TCI scores in a US sample obtained from visitors to a shopping mall, and the authors considered that they were representative of the general population. Our subjects were patients or volunteers selected because they had a sibling willing to participate in the study. We have commented elsewhere6 that our method of control proband selection may have led to the inclusion of particularly altruistic, helpful volunteers and that their scores on the 7 scales of the TCI may have reflected this bias. However, for all but 3 of the scales (harm avoidance, persistence, and self-transcendence), the mean scores for the US sample and the 4 Welsh groups were very similar. For harm avoidance, we suggest that the difference is largely due to the currently depressed subjects contributing to the total for the Welsh group; the mean scores for the healthy control subjects were similar to the US mean scores. Although the mean scores for persistence were only marginally higher in the US compared with the Welsh groups, the mean self-transcendence score for the US population sample was substantially higher than for all 4 Cardiff groups. This may suggest that these US subjects were more religious or spiritual than those from Wales.
The relationship between self-transcendence and personality disorder as conceptualized in the DSM-III-R12 was examined by Svrakic et al,13 who showed that only schizoid personality disorder was significantly (negatively) correlated with self-transcendence scores. The authors concluded that the concepts captured by the self-transcendence scale of the TCI were not incorporated into the traditional personality disorder criteria of the DSM. They suggested that further systematic studies of this scale in other populations would help to clarify its clinical significance.1 The findings of the Cardiff Depression Study support the view that there may be national or cultural differences in scores on this scale that warrant further investigation, although no differences from US self-transcendence scores have been reported by investigators using the TCI in Swedish14 or Spanish15 populations.
Familiality and history of major depression
As we have commented elsewhere,6 the relative risk of reported depression in the D-sibs compared with the C-sibs(5.42) was higher than in most previous studies of major depression.16 This may be attributable in part to the relatively low rates of depression in the C-sibs, who were ascertained via C-probands selected for mental health.
The D-probands had significantly different scores compared with the other 3 groups for cooperativeness, harm avoidance, novelty seeking, and self-directedness. Similar results (higher harm avoidance and lower cooperativeness and self-directedness scores in subjects with depression compared with healthy controls) have been found by others.3,17 Hansenne et al17 found that self-transcendence scores were higher in subjects with depression than healthy controls. Although there were no differences in self-transcendence scores between the 4 subject groups in the Cardiff Depression Study, self-transcendence was significantly, albeit weakly, correlated with BDI score.
Previously, Brown et al18 suggested that higher-order traits such as novelty seeking and reward dependence were relatively independent of mood and anxiety states, whereas harm avoidance reflected changes in mood to a much greater extent. In keeping with these findings, the Cardiff Depression Study has shown highly significant correlations with current mood measures for harm avoidance and has not found any significant differences across the 4 groups of subjects for reward dependence. However, contrary to the suggestion of Brown and colleagues that novelty seeking is independent of current mood, our study found lower novelty-seeking scores in those with depression compared with controls.
The relationship between current mood state and the 7 TCI scales was also reflected in the correlations with BDI. Of the 7 scales, the 4 significantly correlated with BDI were also significantly different for D-probands compared with the other 3 groups. Harm avoidance and self-directedness had high correlations(0.67 and −0.65, respectively), whereas cooperativeness and novelty seeking had more modest correlations with BDI (−0.23 and −0.18, respectively). In addition to these associations with current mood measures, all 7 scales of the TCI were familial, as shown by significant sib-pair correlations.
When never-depressed D-sibs were compared with D-sibs who reported a previous episode of depression, there were no significant differences in scores on 5 of the scales. However, scores for harm avoidance remained significantly higher and scores for reward dependence significantly lower in the group with previous depression. This suggests that the cooperativeness, novelty-seeking, and self-directedness scores in subjects with depression return to the normal range following recovery. For harm avoidance and reward dependence, although clinical recovery leads to some improvement in scores, they do not return to a level similar to never-depressed D-sibs. Elevated harm avoidance scores following recovery from depression have also been noted by Richter et al.4
Vulnerability traits for depression
Our results show that several of the TCI scales are sensitive to a history of depression. To attempt to overcome current or past mood state as a possible confounding factor, we examined 2 groups of never-depressed subjects (D-sibs and C-sibs) who were first-degree relatives of subjects with depression or healthy controls. If there were a genetic relationship between a heritable personality trait and depression, the siblings of subjects with depression who had not been depressed themselves, yet who share on average 50% of their genes with their depressed sibling, should have differed on measures of the trait when compared with the never-depressed siblings of healthy individuals. Although the never-depressed D-sibs did not express the phenotype for depression, they should typically have more depression vulnerability genes than individuals(C-sibs) with no such relationship to a D-proband. Consequently, by comparing these 2 groups, it is possible to begin to disentangle state-dependency effects or the effects of prior episodes of depression from potential heritable vulnerability effects. The results showed significant differences between never-depressed D-sibs and C-sibs for harm avoidance, novelty seeking, reward dependence, and self-directedness. Mean scores for never-depressed D-sibs were higher than for never-depressed C-sibs for harm avoidance and reward dependence and lower for novelty seeking and self-directedness. For harm avoidance, noveltyseeking, and self-directedness, the mean scores of the never-depressed D-sibs were intermediate between those of the D-probands and never-depressed C-sibs. This suggests that these 3 scales have traitlike characteristics related to a familial vulnerability to depression and are further lowered or elevated when the subject is depressed.
In Table 5, we summarize the relationship between TCI profiles that appear to characterize subjects withpresent and past depression or a genetic liability to depression. Our results suggest that the 7 scales fall into 4 main types. The first of these consists of the harm avoidance scale. This scale is highly correlated with current depressed mood, and scores do not return to the normal range with recovery. This poses the following question: Are these elevated harm avoidance scores in the group that recovered from depression due to the effect of "scarring" following the depression? Harm avoidance scores were also elevated in those who had an increased genetic loading for depression but had never been depressed, suggesting that harm avoidance is a trait marker of liability to depression.
The second type consists of novelty seeking and self-directedness, both of which were negatively associated with current mood. The D-sibs with a history of depression did not have higher novelty-seeking or self-directedness scores than D-sibs with no history of depression. However, like harm avoidance, scores on both these scales showed some association with familial liability to depression; never-depressed D-sibs had significantly lower scores than never-depressed C-sibs.
Reward dependence forms the third scale type. Mean reward dependence scores were significantly higher in the never-depressed D-sibs compared with C-sibs. This is opposite to the direction of the scores found in D-sibs who reported a history of depression. Also, reward dependence scores were not correlated with current mood measures. One interpretation of these findings is that reward dependence is influenced only by recovery from depression. Individuals who have recovered from major depression may become less sentimental, less socially attached, and less dependent on the approval of others, the personality characteristics encompassed by the scale.1 On the other hand, the scale may also measure resilience; that is, the characteristics included in the scale may provide some protection against depression in those who have an increased genetic liability to develop this disorder.
The last scale type consists of cooperativeness, self-transcendence, and persistence. Whereas these scales were familial, the results suggest that this familiality is not related to depression. Although cooperativeness was negatively associated and self-transcendence positively associated with current low mood, neither type of score was influenced by a history of depression, nor were there differences from control subjects in never-depressed first-degree relatives of subjects with depression.
Although we could classify our subjects into groups according to whether they were well, depressed at the time of the interview, or had ever been depressed in the past, we have completed TCI questionnaires only for a single time point. In other words, we have made inferences about the relationship between TCI scores and depression on the basis of cross-sectional data.
Using a sib-pair design allows us to comment on familial rather than genetic risk factors. However, a recent clinically based twin study19 and several population based studies (described by Sullivan et al20) found no evidence of shared environmental effects in depression, and genetic studies of personality have been virtually unanimous in finding only nonfamilial environmental effects.21 Therefore, most or all of our observed sibling similarities likely reflect shared genes, not shared environment.
The number of subjects included in the analyses is quite small when divided according to the presence or absence of past depression. Furthermore, we used an exploratory approach to data analysis with the attendant risk of multiple testing. Nevertheless, we found some fairly large group differences and a general pattern of results that is in keeping with broad predictions about TCI scales and their relationship with the familial basis of depression.
We have examined the 7 TCI scales in the 4 subject groups who participated in the Cardiff Depression Study. The study has shown that all 7 scales are familial, which supports the proposal of Cloninger et al1 that the TCI measures enduring and heritable aspects of personality. However, 1 of the scales, self-transcendence, showed a marked difference between our Welsh sample and the published US population scores,1 suggesting strong cultural effects. Consequently, the self-transcendence scale may have cultural or national variations in healthy populations, and this requires further investigation. For all other scales, the mean scores for our Welsh cohort were highly similar to the published US means.
Harm avoidance scores were substantially influenced by current and past depressed state. The scale also showed traitlike characteristics that are probably related to a genetic vulnerability to depression because the D sibs, even those who had never had depression, scored highly. Novelty seeking and self-directedness were negatively correlated with current low mood. Both of these scales also have traitlike characteristics; low scores are related to an increased risk of depression, whereas high scores could be associated with protection against the development of depression. Although reward dependence is not influenced by current mood, these scores are lowered in those with a history of depression and elevated in individuals with a genetic predisposition to depression but who have never been depressed. Consequently, high scores for reward dependence may play a protective role against the development of depression. Finally, the cooperativeness, persistence, and self-transcendence scales appear to have only a limited relationship to the familiality of depression. Future strategies for further examining vulnerability factors for depression could include prospective or longitudinal studies of the siblings of depressed probands who have not yet developed the disorder.
Corresponding author and reprints: Anne Farmer, MD, FRCPsych, Medical Research Council, Social Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Research Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, Camberwell, London SE5 8AF, England(e-mail: A.Farmer@iop.kcl.ac.uk).
Submitted for publication January 22, 2002; final revision received August 19, 2002; accepted October 4, 2002.
This study was funded by a grant from the Wellcome Trust, London.
1.Cloninger
ÌýCRSvrakic
ÌýDMPrzybek
ÌýTRÌýA psychobiological model of temperament and character.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50975-Ìý989
2.Sato
ÌýTNarita
ÌýTHirano
ÌýSKusunoki
ÌýKGoto
ÌýMSakado
ÌýKUehara
ÌýTÌýFactor validity of the temperament and character inventory in patients with major depression.ÌýÌýCompr Psychiatry. 2001;42337-Ìý341
3.Kusunoki
ÌýKSato
ÌýTTaga
ÌýCYoshida
ÌýTKomori
ÌýKNarita
ÌýTHirano
ÌýSOzaki
ÌýNÌýLow novelty-seeking differentiates obsessive-compulsive disorders from major depression.ÌýÌýActa Psychiatr Scand. 2000;101403-Ìý405
4.Richter
ÌýJEisemann
ÌýMRichter
ÌýGÌýTemperament and character during the course of unipolar depression among patients.ÌýÌýEur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2000;25040-Ìý47
5.Katz
ÌýRMcGuffin
ÌýPÌýNeuroticism in familial depression.ÌýÌýPsychol Med. 1987;17155-Ìý161
6.Farmer
ÌýAEHarris
ÌýTRedman
ÌýKSadler
ÌýSMahmood
ÌýAMcGuffin
ÌýPÌýThe Cardiff Depression Study.ÌýÌýBr J Psychiatry. 2000;176150-Ìý155
7.Farmer
ÌýARedman
ÌýKHarris
ÌýTMahmood
ÌýASadler
ÌýSMcGuffin
ÌýPÌýSensation-seeking, life events and depression.ÌýÌýBr J Psychiatry. 2001;178549-Ìý552
8.American Psychiatric Association,ÌýDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition.Ìý Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
9.Wing
ÌýJKBabor
ÌýTBrugha
ÌýTBurke
ÌýJCooper
ÌýJEGiel
ÌýRJablenski
ÌýARegier
ÌýDSartorius
ÌýNÌýSCAN: Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47589-Ìý593
10.Beck
ÌýATWard
ÌýCHMendelson
ÌýMMock
ÌýJErbaugh
ÌýJÌýAn inventory for measuring depression.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4561-Ìý571
11.Mendlowicz
ÌýMVJean-Louis
ÌýGGillin
ÌýJCAkiskal
ÌýHSFurlanetto
ÌýLMRapaport
ÌýMHKelsoe
ÌýJRÌýSociodemographic predictors of temperament and character.ÌýÌýJ Psychiatr Res. 2000;34221-Ìý226
12.American Psychiatric Association,ÌýDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition.Ìý Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1987;
13.Svrakic
ÌýDMWhitehead
ÌýCPrzybeck
ÌýTRCloninger
ÌýRÌýDifferential diagnosis of personality disorders by the seven-factor model of temperament and character.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50991-Ìý999
14.Brandstrom
ÌýSSchlette
ÌýPPrzybeck
ÌýTRLundberg
ÌýMForsgren
ÌýTSigve
ÌýSNylander
ÌýPONilsson
ÌýLGCloninger
ÌýRCAdolfsson
ÌýRÌýSwedish normative data on personality using the Temperament and Character Inventory.ÌýÌýCompr Psychiatry. 1998;39122-Ìý128
15.Gutierrez
ÌýFTorrens
ÌýMBoget
ÌýTMartin-Santos
ÌýRSangorrin
ÌýJPerez
ÌýOSalamero
ÌýMÌýPsychometric properties of the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) questionnaire in a Spanish psychiatric population.ÌýÌýActa Psychiatr Scand. 2001;103143-Ìý147
16.Tsuang
ÌýMTFaraone
ÌýSVÌýThe Genetics of Mood Disorders.Ìý Baltimore, Md Johns Hopkins University Press1990;
17.Hansenne
ÌýMReggers
ÌýJPinto
ÌýEKjiri
ÌýKAjamier
ÌýAAnsseau
ÌýMÌýTemperament and Character Inventory (TCI) and depression.ÌýÌýJ Psychiatr Res. 1999;3331-Ìý36
18.Brown
ÌýSLSvrakic
ÌýDMPrzybeck
ÌýTRCloninger
ÌýCRÌýThe relationship of personality to mood and anxiety states.ÌýÌýJ Psychiatr Res. 1992;26197-Ìý211
19.McGuffin
ÌýPKatz
ÌýRWatkins
ÌýSRutherford
ÌýJÌýA hospital-based twin register of the heritability of DSM-IV unipolar depression.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53129-Ìý136
20.Sullivan
ÌýPFNeale
ÌýMKendler
ÌýKSÌýGenetic epidemiology of major depression: review and meta-analysis.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 2000;1571552-Ìý1562
21.Plomin
ÌýRDeFries
ÌýJCMcClearn
ÌýGEMcGuffin
ÌýPÌýBehavioral Genetics.Ìý4th New York, NY Worth Publishers2001;