BackgroundÌý
Bright light therapy is the recommended treatment for winter seasonal affective disorder (SAD). However, the studies with the best placebo controls have not been able to demonstrate that light treatment has a benefit beyond its placebo effect.
MethodsÌý
Ninety-six patients with SAD completed the study. Patients were randomly assigned to 1 of 3 treatments for 4 weeks, each 1.5 hours per day: morning light (average start time about 6 AM), evening light (average start about 9 PM), or morning placebo (average start about 6 AM). The bright light (≈6000 lux) was produced by light boxes, and the placebos were sham negative-ion generators. Depression ratings using the Structured Interview Guide for the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale, SAD version (SIGH-SAD) were performed weekly.
ResultsÌý
There were no differences among the 3 groups in expectation ratings or mean depression scores after 4 weeks of treatment. However, strict response criteria revealed statistically significant differences; after 3 weeks of treatment morning light produced more of the complete or almost complete remissions than placebo. By 1 criterion (24-item SIGH-SAD score <50% of baseline and ≤8), 61% of the patients responded to morning light, 50% to evening light, and 32% to placebo after 4 weeks of treatment.
ConclusionsÌý
Bright light therapy had a specific antidepressant effect beyond its placebo effect, but it took at least 3 weeks for a significant effect to develop. The benefit of light over placebo was in producing more of the full remissions.
THE SHORTER photoperiod and decrease in sunlight exposure experienced by people living in temperate and higher latitudes during the winter1-3 is hypothesized to be the main trigger for winter depression or seasonal affective disorder (SAD).4 Supplementation with bright artificial light is therefore logical and is also the medically recommended treatment of choice.5,6 There is no doubt that bright light therapy can reduce and even eliminate the symptoms of winter SAD. However, it has remained difficult to demonstrate that bright light has an antidepressant effect beyond its placebo effect. One reason stems from the problems inherent in finding an appropriate placebo control for light box treatment.
Many researchers used dim light as the placebo control for bright light. However, even in the earliest study,7 most patients predicted that bright light would be more helpful. Because expectations for improvement are believed to account for a large component of placebo effects8-12 and placebo effects account for a large component of antidepressant effects,13 the superiority of bright light could have been entirely due to placebo effects. Another strategy has been to use evening light as the placebo control for morning light.14 However, some investigators have shown that evening light can be as antidepressant as morning light,15 limiting the usefulness of evening light as an inert placebo.
A recent study successfully implemented an inert placebo control, a light box that emitted no visible light, but had a pilot light and a hum when turned "on."16 Patients were told that the purpose of the experiment was to study infrared, nonvisible light. The deception was successful; there was no difference in expectations between the patients assigned to bright light and those assigned to placebo. Unfortunately, there was also no difference in antidepressant response.
Several years ago, we used an inert (deactivated) negative ion generator as the placebo control for bright light treatment.17 To make ion treatment plausible, patients were given an information packet including the statement that, in general, people are exposed to more negative ions in the summer as well as to more light. Patients were treated with 1 hour of morning bright light and 1 hour of morning placebo in a balanced crossover design. Expectation ratings showed that the patients expected their light box to make them feel slightly better than their generator. Despite this slight advantage for light, the benefit of light treatment over placebo was not statistically significant.
For the current study, we improved the design to increase the chances of finding a significant effect of bright light treatment, should it exist. We increased the daily duration of light treatment from 1.0 to 1.5 hours, and increased the weeks of light treatment from 2 to 4. We coached the staff to be enthusiastic and positive about both treatments, as opposed to the rather neutral and pessimistic attitude displayed in our previous study. We also changed to a simpler parallel design, instead of a crossover design, and increased the sample size. An evening light group was included to address the controversy about whether time of day of treatment is important. We also wanted to increase the expectations for the generator treatment to be more similar to those for the light treatment. Therefore, we had more impressive ion generators built and to increase the "dose" we gave each patient 2 units instead of 1.
Patients were recruited through advertisements and the local media. They were diagnosed by the usual criteria for SAD,7 but in addition we required the "atypical" symptoms of increased appetite or weight and increased sleep. Most SAD patients have these symptoms, and this provided a more homogeneous and more typical sample. Patients also had to score 21 or more on the first 24 items of the Structured Interview Guide for the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale, SAD Version (SIGH-SAD),18 ie, the original 17-item Hamilton Depression Scale plus 7 atypical items, the 24 items that were intended to rate severity. All patients were free of psychotropic medications for several months and none had previously tried bright light or negative ion treatment. None had any complicating medical conditions based on medical histories, physical examinations, and blood and urine evaluations. Patient characteristics are listed in Table 1.
The ion generators were shiny black cylinders, 32 cm in height, 15 cm in diameter, with 3 small lights on the front that changed rapidly between red and green. Because the negative ion mechanism made a soft hissing sound, a white-noise generator was added to active and placebo generators. The patient consent forms explained that they might receive active or deactivated generators, but that they would not be able to tell which type they had received. Two generators were set up on a desk or table in front of the patient, 38.1 cm apart, with 38.1 cm between the patient and each generator.
The light box was 65 cm wide and 43.5 cm tall (Apollo Light Systems, Orem, Utah). It contained 6 horizontally mounted cool-white fluorescent lamps. Patients sat at a table or desk with the light box directly in front of them at a distance of 38.1 cm. They usually read during the treatment time, which produced an illuminance of about 6000 lux.
The 5-week protocol consisted of a baseline week followed by 4 consecutive treatment weeks. Patients were told that they would be randomly assigned to 1 of 4 treatments: morning generator, evening generator, morning light, or evening light. All treatments were 1.5 hours in duration. The morning treatments were taken as soon as possible after waking and the evening treatments were before bed, with 1 hour maximum between the end of treatment and bedtime.
Sleep schedules were negotiated with each patient, and were followed during the 5-week study, including weekends. The wake-up time had to be earlier than usual, to make time for the morning treatments. Even patients assigned to evening treatments had to wake up earlier because they were asked to stay indoors for the first 1.5 hours after waking. This was to help us find a difference between morning and evening treatments, if it existed. Bedtime fell within a 1-hour window so that patients were allowed 7 to 8 hours in bed. Naps were permitted within a 6-hour window in the middle of the waking period. Compliance with the sleep schedule was encouraged and monitored by requiring that patients call a time-stamp answering machine or voice mail 3 times each morning, during the 1.5 hours after waking, and by reviewing daily sleep logs at least once per week. Patients were not permitted to drink alcohol during the 5 weeks or to drink caffeine in the 6 hours before their earliest bedtime.
Patients went to 2 different buildings during weekly visits to the medical center. The clinical staff in the Department of Psychiatry, Rush-Presbyterian-St Luke's Medical Center, Chicago, Ill, performed weekly SIGH-SAD ratings, and were blind to the patients' type of equipment and time of day of treatment. The research assistants in the Biological Rhythms Research Laboratory dispensed equipment, collected questionnaires, and monitored compliance to the protocol. They were not blind to type of equipment or time of day of treatment, but they did not know which patients received active generators and which received placebos. This division of functions between the 2 locations helped to maintain the blinding of the SIGH-SAD raters.
Treatment was administered 6 days per week; it was omitted on the weekly visit day. Patients started the protocol in January or February, except for 3 who started in November.
Before the study began patients were given a packet of information from the scientific and lay literature promoting negative ion and light treatment of SAD. At the end of the baseline week, after patients had known their treatment assignment for 1 week, they completed an expectation questionnaire that asked them to rate, on a 7-point scale, how they expected to feel at the end of the 4-week treatment. Possible answers ranged from "will feel as good as summer" (1) to "no change" (7). They also rated how confident they were in that guess, from "very confident" (1) to "not at all confident" (7).
At the end of each week patients completed the 21-item Beck Depression Inventory,19 to which we added 4 atypical items. The 4 items covered increases in appetite, weight, sleep, and sleepiness.
The study protocol was approved by the institutional review board and all patients gave written informed consent before the study began.
We analyzed data from patients assigned to morning light (ML), morning placebo (MP), and evening light (EL). Patients and raters did not know that very few patients were assigned to active ion generators and none were assigned to evening generators. Patients were randomly assigned to the 3 major groups with balancing for sex. Statistical analyses included repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) and log-linear analyses20 for depression scores, and 1-way ANOVAs and Tukey honestly significant difference post-hoc tests for expectation ratings and sleep parameters. In all statistical tests we used a significance level of .05, 2 tailed, unless otherwise specified. Summary statistics are presented as mean±SD.
During the course of 6 winters, 96 patients completed the study (Table 1) and 25 dropped out during the 5-week protocol (ML=8, EL=8, MP=9).
The expectation ratings showed that all groups expected their treatment to be very successful in reducing their depression (ML=3.6±1.1, MP=3.7±1.2, EL=3.3±1.1). All groups were somewhat confident in their prediction (ML=3.7±1.4, MP=3.9±1.3, EL=3.2±1.2). There were no statistically significant differences among the groups for either item, determined by 1-way ANOVAs. There were small correlations between expectations and depression ratings, showing a trend for patients who expected a greater improvement to achieve a greater reduction in their depression (expectation vs SIGH-SAD difference from pre-baseline to treatment; at treatment week 3, r=−0.18, P=.04; at treatment week 4, r=−0.14, P=.09; both with df=94, 1-tailed tests).
Sleep logs were analyzed to determine whether there were differences among groups (Table 2). If there were differences in sleep schedules or other sleep parameters between light and placebo treatments, then these nonspecific factors might contribute to any differences in antidepressant response. One-way ANOVAs were used to compare treatment groups. Within-group comparisons are only descriptive. There were no statistically significant differences among groups in the scheduled wake-up times, which averaged about 6 AM. Actual wake-up time was about an hour or 2 earlier during the study (baseline and treatment) than before the study (pre-baseline), when there were no restrictions on sleep. However, sleep onset times did not change as much as wake-up times. Consequently, nighttime sleep was about an hour less during the study. Even when naps were added to nighttime sleep to yield total sleep time, sleep during the study was still about an hour less than before. The only significant difference between groups was that the ML group reported slightly more sleep during the 4 treatment weeks compared with the EL group (≈20 minutes per day more). There were no significant differences in sleep between the ML and MP groups.
Figure 1 shows that all 3 treatments greatly reduced depression, but that there was very little difference between the treatments. A repeated-measures ANOVA was performed on the difference from baseline scores. The between-subjects factor was treatment group (ML, EL, MP) and the within-subjects factor was time (treatment weeks 1 through 4). There was a significant effect of time (F3,279=61.15, P<.001), but not of treatment group (F2,93=0.28, P=.75). The interaction of group×time was also not significant (F6,279=1.01, P=.42). Thus, patients improved over time regardless of treatment group. Two tests, the Mauchly sphericity test and the Box M, both revealed that the assumption of homogeneous variances was violated. Therefore, it would not be advisable to rely solely on the ANOVAs for statistical significance.
We classified patients as responders if their 24-item SIGH-SAD score decreased to 50% of baseline. The percent responders for the ML, EL, and MP groups were 55%, 56%, and 52% after 3 weeks of treatment, and 67%, 75%, and 48% after 4 weeks of treatment. To determine whether these percentages were significantly different at each treatment week, log-linear analyses were used to compare the proportion of responders and nonresponders among the 3 groups (equivalent to 2×3 χ2 tests). There were no statistically significant differences at either treatment week 3 or treatment week 4.
Significant differences between treatments finally emerged when we classified patients as responders by strict joint criteria designed to identify those with complete or nearly complete remissions (a decrease in SIGH-SAD of 50% and a score ≤8), criteria similar to those used previously.21Figure 2 shows that the percent responders increased as the weeks progressed, and that ML produced the best response rates. At treatment week 4, the percent responders for the ML, EL, and MP groups were 61%, 50%, and 32%, respectively. Log-linear analyses revealed significant differences among treatments; contrast parameters showed that the response rate for ML was higher than for MP at week 3 (z=2.55, P<.05) and at week 4 (z=2.24, P<.05). In addition, there were more responders to ML than EL at each treatment week, but this difference was only significant during treatment week 3 (z=2.13, P<.05). We also analyzed these data by applying even stricter response criteria. Responders had to meet the same criteria for complete remission as mentioned in Figure 2, but they had to meet these criteria in both treatment weeks 3 and 4. By these criteria the response rates for the ML, EL, and MP groups were 55%, 28%, and 16%, respectively. Morning light was significantly better than MP (z=3.05, P<.01) and significantly better than EL (z=2.13, P <.05).
The reason that significant differences between groups emerged when calculating percent responders (Figure 2), but not with mean ratings (Figure 1) can be understood by considering the distributions of scores (Figure 3). The scatterplots show that more patients met the strict joint criteria for remission in the ML group than in the MP or EL groups. The frequency histograms further illustrate how the scores were distributed differently in the 3 groups. The ML distribution was Poisson-like, with scores clustered at the bottom, whereas the MP distribution was more normal. In summary, although the mean scores were similar among the 3 groups (≈10-12), ML produced the greatest number of full remissions.
The results from the Beck depression ratings were similar to the SIGH-SAD ratings. There was little difference among the 3 groups in mean weekly ratings. Log-linear analyses on percent responders by strict criteria showed significantly higher response rates to ML than MP. However, there were no significant differences between ML and EL (Table 3).
Bright light therapy was a better antidepressant for SAD than placebo, but the difference did not reach statistical significance until the third week of treatment. A lag of at least 3 weeks for significant antidepressant effects to develop is consistent with antidepressant drug studies.22 Thus, it seems likely that the reason our previous efficacy study17 and the other light box study with a good placebo control (no light, infrared deception)16 could not demonstrate a difference between light and placebo was because the treatment time was too short. In both cases, the light treatment was only 2 weeks. The infrared deception study16 also included head-mounted light visors, but there was no difference in antidepressant response between light and no-light visors. There have been 4 other visor studies,23-26 all with 1- or 2-week treatments, that did not show a superiority for the presumed active visor despite large sample sizes. These studies used parallel designs, so that patients only saw their own visor and could not tell if it was a placebo. We believe that these 5 studies16,23-26 are the best efficacy studies of light therapy published so far, because data showed that patients did not have higher expectations for the presumed active treatment than for the placebo.
However, there have been several SAD light box studies that found bright light more antidepressant than dim light, sometimes dramatically better after only 1 or 2 weeks.7,27-31 It is possible that those differences were primarily produced by differences in expectation/placebo effects because these were light box studies, mostly crossover designs, in which the bright vs dim comparison was obvious. This explanation is supported by an analysis of light box studies showing that responders had higher expectations than nonresponders.32 Furthermore, it is well known that the apparent efficacy of antidepressant treatments decreases as designs are made more sophisticated; eg, with more adequate control treatments and better blinding techniques.10,12,13,33
It has been suggested that a certain amount of positive expectations or placebo effect may be necessary to release the specific pharmacologic effect of a drug or the specific effects of other treatments.34-36 This is one reason we strived to increase the positive expectations of our patients. Thus, as expected, there was a larger placebo response rate in the current study than in our previous study17: 36% vs 22% of patients were responders with placebo (≤50% of baseline with 2 weeks of MP). Similarly, there was a larger response rate to bright light, 49% vs 34% (with 2 weeks of ML). The increase in response rate for light was similar to the increase for placebo. Thus, the increased response rate to light in the current study can be accounted for by the increased placebo component rather than factors specific to light treatment, such as the increase in daily dose from 1.0 to 1.5 hours. Conversely, the low response rate to light in the previous study can be attributed to a smaller placebo component, rather than a deficient dose of light.
In this study, the largest component of the antidepressant response to bright light was the placebo effect, as in antidepressant drug studies.13 The placebo effect is composed of many factors, including anxiety reduction associated with receiving a diagnosis and positive expectations for treatment. In our study, another factor was the regular sleep schedule, which could have functioned as a zeitgeber to stabilize circadian rhythms and improve sleep.29,37,38 The restrictions on drinking alcohol and caffeinated beverages also constitute basic sleep hygiene and may have improved sleep. On the other hand, the strict early sleep schedule produced chronic partial sleep deprivation, which could have exacerbated symptoms, especially fatigue, or conceivably produced an antidepressant effect.39,40 Thus, there are many nonspecific factors that could have contributed to the placebo effect. The fact that we found only small correlations between expectation ratings and antidepressant response makes sense given that patient expectations were only one of the factors influencing antidepressant response. The important principle for an efficacy study is to make sure that all the nonspecific factors, including expectations, are the same in both the active and placebo conditions.
Because we succeeded in creating a placebo treatment that was similar to light treatment in many ways (an electrical device that creates an environmental factor, 1.5 hours of regular sitting time, the early sleep schedule, and so on), and because expectation ratings showed no difference between light and placebo, we are fairly confident that bright light had a specific antidepressant effect beyond its placebo effect. However, our placebo could be criticized for being obviously different from a light box. Our expectation questionnaire, although purposely given by research assistants (not by more "formidable" PhDs) and without any pressure for specific responses, still may not have captured the true feelings of our patients. If they actually had higher expectations for light, that could account for the superior antidepressant effect of light in our study. On the other hand, our design had an advantage over the typical double-blind antidepressant drug studies, in which raters can often tell which patients were given drug by the emergence of side effects.13,33 Our raters had no similar clues to guess which patients had light boxes and which had negative-ion generators.
There has been a controversy about whether ML is a better antidepressant than EL, with some studies showing a superiority for ML,14,41-44 but others showing no difference between ML and EL.15,45-48 We found that ML was better than EL by some measures but not by others; the difference between ML and EL was not very robust. Those differences we did find cannot be attributed to differences in expectations, because these 2 groups had similar expectations. However, patients receiving EL obtained slightly less sleep during the treatment weeks than patients receiving ML. Thus, ML could have produced a slightly better antidepressant response because it advances circadian rhythms and can help patients adapt to an earlier sleep schedule, whereas EL delays rhythms and can make it more difficult to adjust.49 The ML and EL groups also differed in the restrictions on social activities. Patients receiving EL had to stay at home (average time 9:00-10:30 PM) to receive their treatment. This interfered with many social and family activities and could have worked against an antidepressant effect. If we had included an evening placebo group, we might have been able to learn more.
Accepted for publication March 24, 1998.
This research was supported by grant Mh42768 from the National Institutes of Mental Health, Rockville, Md.
Presented in part at the Werner-Gren Center International Symposium on Light and Biological Rhythms in Man, Stockholm, Sweden, September 12, 1992, and the Eighth Annual Meeting of the Society for Light Treatment and Biological Rhythms, Washington, DC, June 3, 1996.
Eva Chernin and Patricia Meaden, PhD, were research coordinators in the Department of Psychiatry, Rush-Presbyterian-St Luke's Medical Center, Chicago, Ill. Other staff in the Department of Psychiatry who helped with recruitment, diagnoses, and SIGH-SAD ratings were James Murakami, Marilyn Perlman, Kay Peterson, Jill M. Richards, and Kelly J. Sherack. William Scheftner, MD, reviewed the laboratory test and physical examination reports made during the screening process. Research assistants in the Biological Rhythms Research Laboratory of the Department of Psychology who saw patients and entered data into the computer were David Anyadike, Linda C. Gallo, Christina Hardway, Erin K. Hoese, Michael P. Mahoney, Paula J. Mitchell, PhD, Trina Moss, Charles Splete, and Robert A. Tell. Larry D. Chait, PhD, suggested using negative ion generators as the placebo treatment. Ron Rehr of the Machine Shop and Phillip Zervos of the Electronic Research Facility of Rush-Presbyterian-St Luke's Medical Center helped design and build the negative ion generators.
Reprints: Charmane I. Eastman, PhD, Biological Rhythms Research Laboratory, Rush-Presbyterian-St Luke's Medical Center, 1653 W Congress Pkwy, Chicago, IL 60612 (e-mail: ceastman@rush.edu).
1.Eastman
ÌýCIÌýNatural summer and winter sunlight exposure patterns in seasonal affective disorder.ÌýÌýPhysiol Behav. 1990;48611-Ìý616
2.Cole
ÌýRJKripke
ÌýDFWisbey
ÌýJMason
ÌýWJGruen
ÌýWHauri
ÌýPJJuarez
ÌýSÌýSeasonal variation in human illumination exposure at two different latitudes.ÌýÌýJ Biol Rhythms. 1995;10324-Ìý334
3.Guillemette
ÌýJHebert
ÌýMPaquet
ÌýJDumont
ÌýMÌýNatural bright light exposure in the summer and winter in subjects with and without complaints of seasonal mood variations.ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1998;44622-Ìý628
4.Lewy
ÌýAJKern
ÌýHARosenthal
ÌýNEWehr
ÌýTAÌýBright artificial light treatment of a manic-depressive patient with a seasonal mood cycle.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1982;1391496-Ìý1498
5.Rosenthal
ÌýNEÌýDiagnosis and treatment of seasonal affective disorder.ÌýÌý´³´¡²Ñ´¡. 1993;2702717-Ìý2720
6.Oren
ÌýDARosenthal
ÌýNEÌýSeasonal affective disorders.ÌýPaykel
ÌýESedÌýHandbook of Affective Disorders New York, NY Churchill Livingstone1992;551-Ìý564
7.Rosenthal
ÌýNESack
ÌýDAGillin
ÌýJCLewy
ÌýAJGoodwin
ÌýFKDavenport
ÌýYMueller
ÌýPSNewsome
ÌýDAWehr
ÌýTAÌýSeasonal affective disorder: a description of the syndrome and preliminary findings with light therapy.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1984;4172-Ìý80
8.Jones
ÌýRAÌýSelf-Fulfilling Prophecies: Social, Psychological, and Physiological Effects of Expectancies.Ìý New York, NY John Wiley & Sons1977;
9.Shapiro
ÌýAKMorris
ÌýLAÌýThe placebo effect in medical and psychological therapies.ÌýGarfield
ÌýSLBergin
ÌýAEedsÌýHandbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change An Empirical Analysis New York, NY John Wiley & Sons1978;369-Ìý410
10.Ross
ÌýMOlson
ÌýJMÌýAn expectancy-attribution model of the effects of placebos.ÌýÌýPsychol Rev. 1981;88408-Ìý437
11.Lundh
ÌýLGÌýPlacebo, belief, and health: a cognitive-emotional model.ÌýÌýScand J Psychol. 1987;28128-Ìý143
12.Eastman
ÌýCIÌýWhat the placebo literature can tell us about light therapy for SAD.ÌýÌýPsychopharmacol Bull. 1990;26495-Ìý504
13.Greenberg
ÌýRPFisher
ÌýSÌýExamining antidepressant effectiveness: findings, ambiguities, and some vexing puzzles.ÌýFisher
ÌýSGreenberg
ÌýRPedsÌýThe Limits of Biological Treatments for Psychological Distress Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence A Erlbaum Associates1989;1-Ìý37
14.Lewy
ÌýAJSack
ÌýRLMiller
ÌýLSHoban
ÌýTMÌýAntidepressant and circadian phase-shifting effects of light.ÌýÌý³§³¦¾±±ð²Ô³¦±ð. 1987;235352-Ìý354
15.Wirz-Justice
ÌýAGraw
ÌýPKrauchi
ÌýKGisin
ÌýBJochum
ÌýAArendt
ÌýJFisch
ÌýH-UBuddeberg
ÌýCPoldinger
ÌýWÌýLight therapy in seasonal affective disorder is independent of time of day or circadian phase.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50929-Ìý937
16.Levitt
ÌýAJWesson
ÌýVAJoffe
ÌýRTMaunder
ÌýRGKing
ÌýEFÌýA controlled comparison of light box and head-mounted units in the treatment of seasonal depression.ÌýÌýJ Clin Psychiatry. 1996;57105-Ìý110
17.Eastman
ÌýCILahmeyer
ÌýHWWatell
ÌýLGGood
ÌýGDYoung
ÌýMAÌýA placebo-control led trial of light treatment for winter depression.ÌýÌýJ Affect Disord. 1992;26211-Ìý222
18.Williams
ÌýJBWLink
ÌýMJRosenthal
ÌýNEAmira
ÌýLTerman
ÌýMÌýStructured Interview Guide for the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale–Seasonal Affective Disorder Version (SIGH-SAD).Ìý New York, NY NY State Psychiatric Institute1992;
19.Beck
ÌýATWard
ÌýCHMendelson
ÌýMMock
ÌýJErbaugh
ÌýJÌýAn inventory for measuring depression.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;453-Ìý63
20.Bishop
ÌýYMMFienberg
ÌýSEHolland
ÌýPWÌýDiscrete Multivariate Analysis.Ìý Cambridge, Mass MIT Press1975;
21.Terman
ÌýMTerman
ÌýJSQuitkin
ÌýFMMcGrath
ÌýPJStewart
ÌýJWRafferty
ÌýBÌýLight therapy for seasonal affective disorder: a review of efficacy.ÌýÌý±·±ð³Ü°ù´Ç±è²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²¹°ù³¾²¹³¦´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1989;21-Ìý22
22.Janicak
ÌýPGDavis
ÌýJMPreskorn
ÌýSHAyd
ÌýFJ
ÌýJrÌýPrinciples and Practice of Psychopharmacotherapy.Ìý Baltimore, Md Williams & Wilkins1997;
23.Rosenthal
ÌýNEMoul
ÌýDEHellekson
ÌýCJOren
ÌýDAFrank
ÌýABrainard
ÌýGCMurray
ÌýMGWehr
ÌýTAÌýA multicenter study of the light visor for seasonal affective disorder: no difference in efficacy found between two different intensities.ÌýÌý±·±ð³Ü°ù´Ç±è²õ²â³¦³ó´Ç±è³ó²¹°ù³¾²¹³¦´Ç±ô´Ç²µ²â. 1993;8151-Ìý160
24.Joffe
ÌýRTMoul
ÌýDELam
ÌýRWLevitt
ÌýAJTeicher
ÌýMHLebegue
ÌýBOren
ÌýDABuchanan
ÌýAGlod
ÌýCAMurray
ÌýMGBrown
ÌýJSchwartz
ÌýPÌýLight visor treatment for seasonal affective disorder: a multicenter study.ÌýÌýPsychiatry Res. 1993;4629-Ìý39
25.Levitt
ÌýAJJoffe
ÌýRTKing
ÌýEÌýDim versus bright red (light-emitting diode) light in the treatment of seasonal affective disorder.ÌýÌýActa Psychiatr Scand. 1994;89341-Ìý345
26.Teicher
ÌýMHGlod
ÌýCAOren
ÌýDASchwartz
ÌýPJLuetke
ÌýCBrown
ÌýCRosenthal
ÌýNEÌýThe phototherapy light visor: more to it than meets the eye.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1995;1521197-Ìý1202
27.Rosenthal
ÌýNESack
ÌýDACarpenter
ÌýCJParry
ÌýBLMendelson
ÌýWBWehr
ÌýTAÌýAntidepressant effects of light in seasonal affective disorder.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1985;142163-Ìý170
28.James
ÌýSPWehr
ÌýTASack
ÌýDAParry
ÌýBLRosenthal
ÌýNEÌýTreatment of seasonal affective disorder with light in the evening.ÌýÌýBr J Psychiatry. 1985;147424-Ìý428
29.Kohler
ÌýWKPelzer
ÌýASchmidt
ÌýKPCarella
ÌýABPflug
ÌýBÌýBright light and dim light in the therapy of depression: effects on the circadian system and clinical results.ÌýÌý±Ê³ó²¹°ù³¾²¹³¦´Ç±è²õ²â³¦³ó¾±²¹³Ù°ù²â. 1989;22202
30.Winton
ÌýFCorn
ÌýTHuson
ÌýLWFraney
ÌýCArendt
ÌýJCheckley
ÌýSAÌýEffects of light treatment upon mood and melatonin in patients with seasonal affective disorder.ÌýÌýPsychol Med. 1989;19585-Ìý590
31.Magnusson
ÌýAKristbjarnarson
ÌýHÌýTreatment of seasonal affective disorder with high-intensity light: a phototherapy study with an Icelandic group of patients.ÌýÌýJ Affect Disord. 1991;21141-Ìý147
32.Terman
ÌýMAmira
ÌýLTerman
ÌýJSRoss
ÌýDCÌýPredictors of response and nonreponse to light treatment for winter depression.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1996;1531423-Ìý1429
33.Fisher
ÌýSGreenberg
ÌýRPÌýHow sound is the double-blind design for evaluating psychotropic drugs?.ÌýÌýJ Nerv Ment Dis. 1993;181345-Ìý350
34.Uhlenhuth
ÌýEHCanter
ÌýANeustadt
ÌýJOPayson
ÌýHEÌýThe symptomatic relief of anxiety with meprobamate, phenobarbital and placebo.ÌýÌý±Ê²õ²â³¦³ó¾±²¹³Ù°ù²â. 1959;115905-Ìý910
35.Shapiro
ÌýAKÌýPlacebo effects in medicine, psychotherapy, and psychoanalysis.ÌýBergin
ÌýAEGarfield
ÌýSLedsÌýHandbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change An Empirical Analysis New York, NY John Wiley & Sons1971;439-Ìý473
36.Grunbaum
ÌýAÌýThe placebo concept in medicine and psychiatry.ÌýÌýPsychol Med. 1986;1619-Ìý38
37.Monk
ÌýTHPetrie
ÌýSRHayes
ÌýAJKupfer
ÌýDJÌýRegularity of daily life in relation to personality, age, gender, sleep quality and circadian rhythms.ÌýÌýJ Sleep Res. 1994;3196-Ìý205
38.Manber
ÌýRBootzin
ÌýRRAcebo
ÌýCCarskadon
ÌýMAÌýThe effect of regularizing sleep-wake schedules on daytime sleepiness.ÌýÌý³§±ô±ð±ð±è. 1996;19432-Ìý441
39.Schilgen
ÌýBTolle
ÌýRÌýPartial sleep deprivation as therapy for depression.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1980;37267-Ìý271
40.Vogel
ÌýGWVogel
ÌýFMcAbee
ÌýRSThurmond
ÌýAJÌýImprovement of depression by REM sleep deprivation.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1980;37247-Ìý253
41.Sack
ÌýRLLewy
ÌýAJWhite
ÌýDMSinger
ÌýCMFireman
ÌýMJVandiver
ÌýRÌýMorning vs evening light treatment for winter depression.ÌýÌýArch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47343-Ìý351
42.Avery
ÌýDHKhan
ÌýADager
ÌýSRCox
ÌýGBDunner
ÌýDLÌýBright light treatment of winter depression: morning versus evening light.ÌýÌýActa Psychiatr Scand. 1990;82335-Ìý338
43.Avery
ÌýDHKhan
ÌýADager
ÌýSRCohen
ÌýSCox
ÌýGBDunner
ÌýDLÌýMorning or evening bright light treatment of winter depression? the significance of hypersomnia..ÌýÌýBiol Psychiatry. 1991;29117-Ìý126
44.Terman
ÌýJSTerman
ÌýMSchlager
ÌýDRafferty
ÌýBRosofsky
ÌýMLink
ÌýMJGallin
ÌýPFQuitkin
ÌýFMÌýEfficacy of brief, intense light exposure for treatment of winter depression.ÌýÌýPsychopharmacol Bull. 1990;263-Ìý11
45.Hellekson
ÌýCJKline
ÌýJARosenthal
ÌýNEÌýPhototherapy for seasonal affective disorder in Alaska.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1986;1431035-Ìý1037
46.Meesters
ÌýYJansen
ÌýJHCLambers
ÌýPABouhuys
ÌýALBeersma
ÌýDGMvan den Hoofdakker
ÌýRHÌýMorning and evening light treatment of seasonal affective disorder: response, relapse and prediction.ÌýÌýJ Affect Disord. 1993;28165-Ìý177
47.Meesters
ÌýYJansen
ÌýJHCBeersma
ÌýDGMBouhuys
ÌýALvan den Hoofdakker
ÌýRHÌýLight therapy for seasonal affective disorder: the effects of timing.ÌýÌýBr J Psychiatry. 1995;166607-Ìý612
48.Lafer
ÌýBSachs
ÌýGSLabbate
ÌýI-AThibault
ÌýARosenbaum
ÌýJFÌýPhototherapy for seasonal affective disorder: a blind comparison of three different schedules.ÌýÌýAm J Psychiatry. 1994;1511081-Ìý1083
49.Eastman
ÌýCIÌýCircadian rhythms and bright light: recommendations for shift work.ÌýÌýWork Stress. 1990;4245-Ìý260