Background
Eye tracking deficits are one of a few widely validated behavioral markers of risk for schizophrenia. Recently, it has been proposed that antisaccade performance may also constitute a marker of schizophrenia risk. This study investigated whether eye tracking and antisaccade deficits could be found in another population with putative liability to schizophrenia—nonclinical subjects with elevated scores on a psychometric index of perceptual aberrations.
Methods
Subjects were 55 university students who received either high or normal scores on the Perceptual Aberration Scale, a measure of schizotypy indexing body image and perceptual distortions. Subjects completed a smooth pursuit eye tracking task and an antisaccade task. Eye movements were monitored using an infrared limbus tracker.
Results
Subjects with high Perceptual Aberration Scale scores (putative "schizotypes") had lower pursuit quality and a lower percentage correct on the antisaccade task than the controls. The 2 groups did not differ in antisaccade or error latencies. The increase in antisaccade errors in the schizotypes was accounted for almost entirely by an increase in perseverative errors, but virtually no difference between groups on random errors. Antisaccade performance was significantly related to pursuit quality.
Conclusions
Subjects with elevated Perceptual Aberration Scale scores have performance deficits on oculomotor tasks that have been linked to latent liability to schizophrenia, namely, smooth pursuit and antisaccade performance. The antisaccade errors in the schizotype group were primarily perseverations, a behavioral pattern often associated with frontal lobe dysfunction and observed in the performance of schizophrenic patients.
IN POPULATIONS at increased risk for schizophrenia, a notable proportion of individuals show abnormalities on cognitive, perceptual, or motor tasks—abnormalities that are similar to those found in schizophrenic patients. At-risk populations also have elevated scores on subclinical symptoms related to schizophrenia, such as thought disorder, abnormal perceptual experiences, and social withdrawal.1,2 The behavioral and perceptual anomalies that occur at higher rates in populations at risk for schizophrenia have been called "behavioral markers" of latent liability to the disorder.3 These markers are the subject of extensive study, both because they may enhance the power of genetic studies of schizophrenia, and because an understanding of their neural basis may elucidate the nature of brain abnormalities relevant to schizophrenia.
Eye tracking dysfunctions (ETD) are one of the most widely validated of the behavioral markers.4 Eye tracking dysfunctions are found at elevated rates in schizophrenic patients; relatives of schizophrenic patients have elevated rates of ETD, but relatives of bipolar patients do not. Patients with schizotypal personality, but not patients with nonspectrum personality disorders, have poorer pursuit quality than normal controls (NCs).5
Recently, deficits on the antisaccade task have been proposed as another marker of latent liability.6,7 In the antisaccade task (Figure 1), the subject fixates a central dot that steps at random to the left or right. When the target steps into the periphery, the subject's task is to look away from the target to the opposite hemifield. Numerous reports8-10 have been published of elevated rates of antisaccade errors in schizophrenic patients. Recently a robust increase in error rates6,7 and an increase in antisaccade latencies10 have been reported in healthy first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients. These findings in a high-risk population suggest that antisaccade deficits are unrelated to the disease process but may be related to latent liability for the disorder.
Three strategies are used to define populations at risk for schizophrenia: (1) a biological relationship to an affected person (eg, first-degree relative); (2) an Axis II diagnosis of cluster A personality disorders (eg, schizotypal personality disorder); and (3) deviance on an established laboratory measure or psychometric marker of schizotypy (eg, indexes of schizotypic phenomena).11 We undertook this study to determine if at-risk subjects ascertained by the third strategy would show the same pattern of eye movement deficits seen in schizophrenic patients and their first-degree relatives. We used a well-established measure of schizotypy, the Perceptual Aberration Scale,12 to select nonpsychotic schizotypic individuals for study. We hypothesized that the group with elevated Perceptual Aberration Scale scores would have poorer smooth pursuit quality and worse performance on the antisaccade task than nonschizotypic controls. Because it has been proposed that both ETD and poor antisaccade performance may reflect deficient inhibitory control of saccades,13 we evaluated the relationship between smooth pursuit quality and antisaccade performance.
Subjects were undergraduates at Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, recruited from an entering class using an epidemiological survey method. We have described these procedures extensively elsewhere.14 Of the 2000 students approached, 1684 subjects completed the 250-item questionnaire, which included the 35 items from the Perceptual Aberration Scale. The 84.2% response rate was consistent with representative sampling.15 Two percent of the subjects were excluded for having elevated scores on an infrequency scale used to detect random responding.16 Subjects were assigned to the high Perceptual Aberration Scale (HPA) group, also referred to as "schizotypic," if they scored more than 2 SD above the mean of their sex, and to the NC group if they scored within 0.5 SD of their sex.11,17 Of the 1646 students, 76 students met criteria for inclusion in the HPA group, and 1332 subjects met criteria for the NC group. Fifty cases were selected randomly from those eligible for each group as possible participants. The consent rate did not differ between HPA and NC subjects. Thirty-one HPA and 24 NC subjects yielded scoreable data on the eye tracking and antisaccade tasks (described in the "Eye Movement Measurement" section). The mean (±SD) Perceptual Aberration Scale score for the HPA group was 19.77±6.34; for the NC group the mean was 0.77±0.99. Demographic characteristics are given in Table 1.
The Perceptual Aberration Scale is a 35-item, true-false, self-report inventory of body image and perceptual disturbances conceptually related to schizophrenia-related psychopathology.12 It includes items like "Now and then when I look in the mirror, my face seems quite different than usual" (keyed true).12 Internal consistency analyses typically yield α coefficients around 0.90 and high test-retest reliability (r=0.75). In nonclinical university samples, individuals with high Perceptual Aberration Scale scores exhibit many of the neurocognitive abnormalities observed in schizophrenic patients and high-risk populations.18-22 Subjects with high Perceptual Aberration Scale scores also have increased levels of clinically significant schizotypal symptoms23 and elevated risk for treated schizophrenia among first-degree relatives.24
Psychological State Measures
The Beck Depression Inventory25 was used to measure depressive and dysphoric symptoms. The State Trait Anxiety Inventory26 measured state and trait anxiety. The self-administered computerized version of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule was used to screen for lifetime history of schizophrenia, schizophreniform illness, mania, and dysthymia.27 The Diagnostic Interview Schedule demonstrates excellent sensitivity for these disorders.28 Each subject provided written consent to obtain access to his or her official Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT) verbal and quantitative scores. These were used as indexes of the subjects' general level of intellectual functioning.
Oculomotor recordings were obtained in a darkened room. Eye movements were monitored using a commercially available software package (Optokinetograph System, Eye and Brain Technology Inc, Thessalonika, Greece). Each subject sat 57cm in front of a computer monitor; the subject's head was stabilized in a custom-made chin rest. Infrared light-emitting diodes and sensors, mounted in ophthalmologic eyeglass frames, were positioned at the level of the limbus in each eye. Subjects completed a 6-point calibration across 24°, repeated before each trial of data collection. After calibration, eye position data were represented linearly across a visual angle of ±12°. Eye movement recordings were digitized at 250 Hz. Saccades were defined as eye movements with a velocity 100% greater than target velocity. This criterion is sensitive to corrective saccades superimposed on smooth pursuit with amplitudes as small as 0.5°. For the antisaccade task, saccades were defined as having a minimum velocity of 8 degrees per second and a minimum duration of 18 milliseconds. The minimum amplitude was 3° and the minimum latency was 100 milliseconds. Saccades were automatically identified by our software (Optokinetograph System) and traces were visually inspected to confirm correct categorization. Testing was done blind to group membership.
Smooth pursuit stimuli and data collection
The target, a 0.5° square, moved with a triangle wave velocity profile at 8 degrees per second across a visual angle of 20°. Eight cycles were collected.
Antisaccade stimuli and data collection
Subjects fixated a square subtending 1°×1° in visual angle at the center of the screen (Figure 1). After a random interval, the target stepped horizontally to a position 12° to the left or right of the fixation point. The direction of the target step was pseudorandom. The screen then went blank for 1000 milliseconds during which the subject's task was not to look toward the target, but to look in the opposite direction. Subjects were instructed that although they were to respond quickly, it was more important to be correct in the direction of the saccade than to be fast. Each subject was given 15 practice trials with the experimenter providing feedback. Subjects then performed 45 trials of the antisaccade task. No feedback was given during testing.
Data reduction, scoring and statistical analyses
The pursuit traces were evaluated independently by 2 raters (G.O'D. and P.S.H.) blind to subject identity and group membership. Traces were assigned a score of normal (3), mixed (2), or abnormal (1). (Mixed was defined as a pursuit trace in which several normal cycles were found, along with clearly abnormal cycles.) After interrater reliability was determined, the scores assigned by the 2 raters were averaged to create a mean pursuit quality score for each subject. Global ratings of pursuit quality, such as qualitative ratings, have been reported to be better than specific pursuit measures, such as pursuit gain, at discriminating schizophrenic patients and their first-degree relatives from NCs.29,30 Qualitative ratings of pursuit are reliable across laboratories,30 and are highly correlated with other global measures such as root mean square error and log signal-to-noise ratio.29 The distribution of the pursuit quality scores was, as expected, significantly nonnormal (SPSS, Chicago, Ill, Kolmogorov-Smirnov procedure z=1.4, P =.04). Therefore nonparametric statistics were used.
Dependent variables were percent correct, latency to antisaccades and to errors, and percent of perseverative errors (PEs) and nonperseverative errors (NPEs). Antisaccade errors were corrected by the subjects, indicating that they understood the task. The percent correct score was calculated as follows: (number of correct antisaccades)/(number of correct antisaccades+number of reflexive saccades [errors]).
We were interested to determine whether subjects' errors reflected a tendency to perseverate, a response pattern associated with frontal dysfunction.31 Perseverative errors were operationalized as errors that represented a continuation of a motor set that was established in the trials immediately preceding the error. The motor pattern had to have been executed consecutively twice immediately prior to the error. This definition yielded 2 types of PEs, as follows: (1) a reflexive saccade (error) in the same direction as the saccades made on the previous 2 trials (ie, left-left-left [error] or right-right-right [error]); and (2) an inappropriate continuation of a pattern of alternation that had been executed consecutively twice in the preceding trials (ie, left-right-left-right-left [error], or right-left-right-left-right [error]). If the error represented a change from, rather than a continuation of, the established motor set, or if no motor set had been established in the previous trials, the errors were classified as NPE. Of the 45 trials, 15 required that the subject abandon an established motor set to make a correct response, while 30 trials required responses that were consistent with the established motor set or were trials in which no momentary motor set had been established. The percent PE score was calculated as follows:(number of PEs)/(15, ie, number of trials in which a PE could be made).
The percent NPE score was calculated as follows:(number of NPEs)/(30, ie, number of trials in which an NPE could be made).
The trials that contributed to PE and NPE scores were classified a priori, assuming a correct pattern of responses in the trials immediately preceding. Because this classification would be incorrect if the subject also made an error in the immediately preceding trials, a second analysis was conducted for each subject using only the direction of the saccades in the previous trials (and not target direction) to define PEs and NPEs. The results were almost identical, and all findings that were significant using one method were significant using the other.
Of 2475 trials collected, 77 (3%) could not be scored for blinks, misfixations, anticipations (latency <100 milliseconds) and failures to respond (saccade <3°). More trials were unscoreable in the HPA group (62) than in the NC group (15) (Mann-Whitney U test, z =−2.45, P=.01). This was attributable to more misfixations and blinks in the HPA group. There was no difference between groups (P>.8) on the number of anticipatory saccades or the number of failures to respond. Between-groups analyses using raw data (number of errors) rather than percent correct were also calculated. All significant between-group results remained significant. Therefore, only the percent correct data are reported below.
The percent correct variable and the percentage of PEs and NPEs were significantly nonnormal in their distribution (Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistic, all P<.01). The usual transformations were attempted but these did not normalize the distributions. Therefore the original data were analyzed using nonparametric statistics. The latency to antisaccades and saccades were calculated separately, with outliers excluded using box-and-whisker plots. Because all latency outliers were made within the time allotted to subjects, they were retained in the error analyses. Antisaccade and error latencies were normally distributed.
The correlation between pursuit quality and percent correct on the antisaccade task was evaluated using the Spearman rank correlation coefficient. Correlations of smooth pursuit and antisaccade indexes with the mental state (anxiety and depression) measures were evaluated using both Spearman and Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients. Partial correlation analysis was used to control for the effects of mental state variables. Between group differences in eye movement performance were evaluated using directional (1-tailed) tests based on our a priori hypotheses and based on published results using these tasks in other high-risk populations, with an α level of .05.4,6,10 All other tests were two tailed.
Subject clinical characteristics
State and Trait Anxiety Scores
State and trait anxiety scores for HPA subjects exceeded those of NC subjects (State Anxiety: HPA, 39.23±10.19; NC, 31.5±6.12; t53 =3.17, P =.002; Trait Anxiety: HPA, 45.26±10.61; NC, 35.13±7.74; t53 =3.94, P <.001). Higher levels of depression (Beck Depression Inventory) were found in the HPA group than in the NC group (HPA, 10.39±5.89; NC, 2.38±2.57; t53 =6.78, P <.001). No subject screened positive for a definite lifetime diagnosis of schizophreniform disorder, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder or dysthymia on the Diagnostic Inventory Schedule.
General Intellectual Level
The 2 subject groups did not differ in either quantitative or verbal domains of the SAT (Table 1).
Smooth Pursuit Performance
The interrater reliability (intraclass correlation coefficient [ICC]) for the rating of pursuit quality was high (0.93). The HPA group had significantly lower pursuit quality than the NC group (1.99±0.73 vs 2.42±0.79, n=51, Mann-Whitney test, z=2.2, P =.01). The modal pursuit score for the HPA group was 1 (abnormal), and the median was 2 (mixed); for the NC group, both the mode and the median pursuit scores were 3 (normal).
Visual field had no effect on percent correct or latency (P>.2). Therefore, results for all trials were combined. The HPA group had significantly lower percent correct on the antisaccade task (0.94±0.06 vs 0.98±0.02, Mann-Whitney test, z=−2.12, P =.02). One quarter of the HPA group made 4 or more errors; no NC subject did.
The groups did not differ in latency to an antisaccade (NC, 266.3±40.4; HPA, 261.8±45.3; t53 =0.38, P =.35) or latency to a reflexive error (NC, 225.3±59.7; HPA, 206.5±57.3; t34=0.95, P =.17)(Figure 2). The latency to an antisaccade was, as expected, significantly longer than the latency to a reflexive error (antisaccade, 259.46±48.4; saccade latency, 213.79±58.15; matched-pair t35 =4.1, P <.001).
High Perceptual Aberrational Scale subjects made significantly more PEs than did NCs (9.5%±10.2% vs 2.2%±3.8%, Mann-Whitney test, z=2.89, n=55, P=.002). This corresponds to an effect size (ϕ) of 0.39. The 2 groups did not differ on NPEs (HPA, 3.3%±4.4% vs NC, 2.1%±3.1%, Mann-Whitney test, z=0.99, n=55, P =.16). Only subjects in the HPA group were more likely to make an error when a correct response required that they change motor set (PE, 9.5%±10.2% vs NPE, 3.3%±4.3%, matched-pair t30 =4.16, P <.001 (effect size d of .78 [Cohen's d for matched pair t test]); for NC group, error rate was unaffected by the requirement to change motor set (PE, 2.2%±3.8% vs NPE, 2.1%±3.1%, t23=0.13, P =.45 [effect size d of .03]). There was no difference in latency to the 2 types of errors (PE, 194.89±53.19; NPE, 191.64±7.8, matched-pair t13=0.51, P =.62).
Correlational Analyses for Antisaccade Performance and Pursuit Quality
Pursuit quality was significantly correlated with percent correct on the antisaccade task (Spearman rank ρ=0.33, n=51, P =.02), and with PE (ρ=−0.32, n=51, P =.01) and NPE (ρ=−0.27, n=51, P =.03).
Correlations of Anxiety, Depression, and SAT Scores With Oculomotor Performance
The Spearman and Pearson correlations between oculomotor scores, the clinical measures and general intellectual functioning were essentially identical. We therefore report only the Pearson correlations along with the relevant partial correlation analyses. Smooth pursuit quality was not significantly correlated with any of the mental state variables, nor was it related to the SAT scores. The antisaccade percent correct, PE and NPE indexes were uncorrelated with anxiety measures and the SAT scores. The scores on the Beck Depression Inventory were significantly correlated (P=.04) with antisaccade percent correct (r=−0.29) and with antisaccade PE and NPE (both correlations=0.29). To examine whether the relationship between antisaccade performance and group membership would remain if the effect of depression were controlled for, we conducted partial correlation analyses. Controlling for depression, the associations between group status and antisaccade PE (partial r =0.31, P=.01) and antisaccade percent correct (partial r=−0.22, P =.06) remained; the association between group membership and NPE did not (partial r =−.03, P=.41). The partial correlations suggest that the association between group membership (ie, schizotypy status) and PE particularly, but also antisaccade percent correct, are not solely a function of depression.
Our results demonstrate that subjects identified as schizotypic (or "hypothetically psychosis prone") by the Perceptual Aberration Scale have a pattern of smooth pursuit and saccadic deficits similar to that observed in schizophrenic patients and their first-degree relatives. Furthermore, the smooth pursuit and antisaccade performance deficits do not seem to be the result of third-variable confounds such as anxiety, depression, or general intellectual ability differences between the groups. A relationship between schizotypal symptoms and antisaccade error rate has been reported in 2 other at-risk populations, first-degree relatives (N=29, r=0.33, P <.08) and clinically diagnosed schizotypal patients.10 To our knowledge, this is the first study to report on antisaccade performance in subjects selected using the psychometric approach to schizotypy.
In our study, the difference in the percent correct scores between HPA and NC groups is accounted for almost exclusively by an increase in the number of PEs in the HPA group. When a correct response required a change in motor set established in the previous trials, HPA subjects showed a 187% increase in error rates over the trials in which no change was required; NC subjects showed only a 4.5% increase in errors. This pattern of errors, in which the subject is unusually vulnerable to proactive interference from previous trials and has difficulty changing sets when momentary, irrelevant response tendencies have been established, is characteristic of monkeys with prefrontal lesions.31 Previous studies of schizophrenic patients have linked antisaccade errors and perseverative tendencies on the Wisconsin Card Sort.32,33 To our knowledge, however, this has not previously been shown in schizotypal or other at-risk populations.
Other studies of university students with elevated Perceptual Aberration Scale scores have also reported performance deficits consistent with a possible frontal impairment, eg, deficits in sustained attention,19 executive functioning,21 and spatial working memory.20 These tasks, like the antisaccade task,34,35 have been shown to activate frontal-striatal-thalamic circuitry in positive emission tomography studies.36-38 Although our study was specifically interested in prefrontal dysfunction, dysfunction elsewhere in the oculomotor circuit may yield similar deficits.39,40 Clarification of the substrates of ETD and antisaccade deficits in vulnerable populations await in vivo imaging investigations. We are conducting such an investigation in first-degree relatives.41
No differences in reaction times were observed between the HPA and NC groups. Schizophrenic patients generally have longer reaction times than NCs.42 This has been demonstrated in one study of first-degree relatives as well.10 However, in that study, the relatives were significantly older than NCs. Two other studies found no difference in antisaccade latency in first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients compared with NCs, and one study found no difference in predictive saccade latencies.6,7 Thus, latency differences on voluntary tasks are not as robust in populations at risk for schizophrenia as they are in the patients themselves; they may be related to the disease process or its treatment rather than liability.
Our study found a higher percent correct in the antisaccade task than reported in some other studies.6,10 This is likely due to small differences in the paradigm used. Studies that use, as we did, both a practice session with feedback and a single amplitude stimulus report error rates in the NC group consistent with those reported here.9,43,44 The antisaccade paradigm used in this study was shown in a previous postive emission tomography study34 to significantly activate frontal eye fields relative to saccades, and to yield antisaccades with significantly longer latencies than saccades; thus, even at low error rates, the task makes significant demands on the voluntary control of eye movements.
Our study also demonstrates that schizotypic subjects identified with the Perceptual Aberration Scalehave reliable deficits in pursuit quality. Our findings are consistent with prior studies that have found that university students with ETD have elevated scores on a variety of measures of schizotypal symptoms.45 Siever et al,5,46 studying clinically identified schizotypal subjects, found that ETD were significantly more common in schizotypal subjects than in NCs, but that the symptom that best predicted pursuit quality was social withdrawal. The association between deficitlike symptoms and ETD has been noted by other investigators.47-50 Nevertheless, several studies have reported a significant relationship between ETD and "psychotic-like" symptoms and/or perceptual abnormalities in clinical and normal populations.51-54 For example, scores on the Perceptual Aberration Scale are significantly associated with pursuit quality among schizophrenic patients55 and with pursuit variability among undergraduates.56
One limitation of our study is that we selected our groups using an imperfect measure of schizotypy: the Perceptual Aberration Scale is thought to generate an admixture of schizotypes and an unknown proportion of individuals who experience perceptual anomalies for reasons unrelated to schizophrenia risk. Although the Perceptual Aberration Scale is a fallible marker of schizophrenia liability, as yet there is no marker in any domain of schizophrenia, including genetics, neurobiology, psychophysiology, and psychometrics, that is capable of specifically detecting liability for schizophrenia with complete fidelity. Nevertheless, we believe this instrument detects a significant number of individuals liable to schizophrenia. The body of data that support this position, reviewed at length elsewhere,57 are not yet as robust for other psychometric measures of schizotypy.
On 10-year clinical follow-up, individuals initially selected for high Perceptual Aberration Scale scores revealed a variety of psychiatric outcomes, with most, but not all, of a schizophrenia-related nature.1 Clearly, not all those scoring high on the Perceptual Aberration Scale go on to develop schizophrenia or related illnesses. However, such heterogeneity of outcomes is also seen in the clinical follow-up of children at risk for schizophrenia. Nevertheless, the inclusion of valid but fallible quantitative markers can greatly improve the power of genetically oriented investigations.58
A few studies59,60 have failed to document elevated Perceptual Aberration Scale scores among first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients, and another study61 has reported significantly lower scores on the Perceptual Aberration Scale in relatives than in NCs. In agreement with some of the authors of these studies, we believe that the findings of lower Perceptual Aberration Scale scores in relatives may be attributable to a defensive response set. Since each of these studies recruited relatives who knew they were being recruited on the basis of their relationship to a schizophrenic proband, it is possible that they interpreted the intent of the questions differently than NCs. However, in a more direct test of the relevance of high Perceptual Aberration Scale scores for genetic risk for schizophrenia, it was found that nonpsychotic individuals with elevated Perceptual Aberration Scale scores had significantly elevated risk of schizophrenia in their first-degree relatives, but no increase in rates of bipolar or unipolar affective illness.24
Reprints: Gillian A. O'Driscoll, PhD, Department of Psychology, McGill University, Stewart Biological Sciences Bldg, 1205 Dr Penfield Ave, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 1B1.
Accepted for publication May 7, 1998.
This work was supported in part by a Stanley Foundation Fellowship, Arlington, Va, a Sackler Scholarship in Psychobiology, Cambridge, Mass, and grants from the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression, Chicago, Ill, Scottish Rite Schizophrenia Research Program, Lexington, Mass, and an Operating Grant from the Medical Research Council, Ottawa, Ontario (Dr O'Driscoll), by Special Projects Grant from Cornell University, Ithaca, NY (Dr Lenzenweger), and grants MH-45448 (Dr Lenzenweger), MH-31340, MH-31154, MH-44860, and MH-01021 from the National Institute of Mental Health, Rockville, Md (Dr Holzman).
Preliminary results were presented at the International Congress of Schizophrenia Research, Warm Springs, Va, April 1995.
We thank Sohee Park, PhD, and Jill Salem, Natasha Frangopolous, and Sylvia Emmerich for their assistance.
1.Chapman
LJChapman
JPKwapil
TREckblad
MZinser
MCPutatively psychosis-prone subjects 10 years later.J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103171-183
2.Kendler
KSWalsh
DSchizotypal personality disorder in parents and the risk for schizophrenia in siblings.Schizophr Bull. 1995;2147-52
3.Holzman
PSBehavioral markers of schizophrenia useful for genetic studies.J Psychiatr Res. 1992;26427-445
4.Levy
DLHolzman
PSMatthysse
SWMendell
NREye tracking dysfunction and schizophrenia: a critical perspective.Schizophr Bull. 1993;19461-505
5.Siever
LJKeefe
RBernstein
DPCoccaro
EFKlar
HMZemishlany
ZPeterson
AEDavidson
MMahon
THorvath
TMohs
RCEye tracking impairment in clinically identified patients with schizotypal personality disorder.Am J Psychiatry. 1990;147740-745
6.Clementz
BMcDowell
JEZisook
SSaccadic system functioning among schizophrenia patients and their first-degree biological relatives.J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103277-287
7.Katsanis
JKortenkamp
SIacono
WGGrove
WMAntisaccade performance in patients with schizophrenia and affective disorder.J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106468-472
8.Fukushima
JFukushima
KChiba
TTanaka
SDisturbances of voluntary control of saccadic eye movements in schizophrenic patients.Biol Psychiatry. 1988;23670-677
9.Sereno
ABHolzman
PSAntisaccades and smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia.Biol Psychiatry. 1995;37394-401
10.Thaker
GKCassady
SAdami
HMoran
MRoss
DEEye movements in spectrum personality disorders: comparison of community subjects and relatives of schizophrenic patients.Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153362-368
11.Chapman
LJChapman
LPPsychosis proneness.Alpert
Med.Controversies in Schizophrenia Changes and Constancies New York, NY Guilford Press1985;157-172
12.Chapman
LJChapman
LPRaulin
MLBody-image aberration in schizophrenia.J Abnorm Psychol. 1978;87399-407
13.Levin
SFrontal lobe dysfunction in schizophrenia, I: eye movement impairments.J Psychiatr Res. 1984;1827-55
14.Holzman
PSColeman
MLenzenweger
MFLevy
DLMatthysse
SWO'Driscoll
GAPark
SWorking memory deficits, antisaccades and thought disorder in relation to perceptual aberration.Raine
ALencz
TMednick
Seds.
Schizotypal Personality New York, NY Cambridge University Press1995;
Google Scholar 15.Kalton
GIntroduction to Survey Sampling. Beverly Hills, Calif Sage1983;
16.Jackson
DNManual for the Personality Research Form.3rd ed. Port Huron, Mich Research Psychologists Press1984;
17.Lenzenweger
MFPsychometric high-risk paradigm, perceptual aberrations, and schizotypy: an update.Schizophr Bull. 1994;20121-135
18.Balogh
DWMerritt
RDSusceptibility to type-A backward pattern masking among hypothetically psychosis-prone college students.J Abnorm Psychol. 1985;94377-383
19.Lenzenweger
MFCornblatt
BAPutnick
MSchizotypy and sustained attention.J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;10084-89
20.Park
SHolzman
PSLenzenweger
MFIndividual differences in spatial working memory in relation to schizotypy.J Abnorm Psychol. 1995;104355-363
21.Lenzenweger
MFKorfine
LPerceptual aberrations, schizotypy, and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.Schizophr Bull. 1994;20345-357
22.Park
SHolzman
PSGoldman-Rakic
PSSpatial working memory deficits in the relatives of schizophrenic patients.Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52821-828
23.Lenzenweger
MFLoranger
AWPsychosis proneness and clinical psychopathology: examination of the correlates of schizotypy.J Abnorm Psychol. 1989;983-8
24.Lenzenweger
MFLoranger
AWDetection of familial schizophrenia using a psychometric measure of schizotypy.Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46902-907
25.Beck
ATWard
CHMendelsohn
MMock
JEErbaugh
JKAn inventory for measuring depression.Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4561-571
26.Spielberger
CDManual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, Calif Consulting Psychologists Press1983;
27.Robins
LNHelzer
JECroughan
JRatcliff
KSNational Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule: its history, characteristics, and validity.Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38381-389
28.Robins
LNMarcus
SCThe Diagnostic Screening Procedure Writer: a tool to develop individualized screening procedures.Med Care. 1987;25(12 Suppl)S106-S122
29.Friedman
LJesberger
JASiever
LJThompson
PMohs
RMeltzer
HYSmooth pursuit performance in patients with affective disorders or schizophrenia and normal controls: analysis with specific oculomotor measures, RMS error and qualitative ratings.Psychol Med. 1995;25387-403
30.Keefe
RSESilverman
JMMohs
RCSiever
LJHarvey
PDFriedman
LLees Roitman
SEDuPre
RLSmith
CJSchmeidler
JDavis
KLEye tracking, attention, and schizotypal symptoms in nonpsychotic relatives of patients with schizophrenia.Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54169-176
31.Fuster
JMThe Prefrontal Cortex. New York, NY Raven Press1980;
32.Crawford
TJHaeger
BKennard
CReveley
MAHenderson
LSaccadic abnormalities in psychotic patients, I: neuroleptic-free psychotic patients.Psychol Med. 1995;25461-471
33.Rosse
RBSchwartz
BLKim
SYDeutsch
SICorrelation between antisaccade and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia.Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150333-335
34.O'Driscoll
GAAlpert
NMMatthysse
SMLevy
DLRauch
SLHolzman
PSFunctional neuroanatomy of antisaccade eye movements investigated with positron emission tomography.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1995;92925-929
Google Scholar 35.Sweeney
JAMintun
MAKwee
SWiseman
MBBrown
DLRosenberg
DRCarl
JRPositron emission tomography study of voluntary saccadic eye movements and spatial working memory.J Neurophysiol. 1996;75454-468
36.Buchsbaum
MSNuechterlein
KHHaier
RJWu
JSicotte
NHazlett
EAsarnow
RGlucose metabolic rate in normals and schizophrenics during the Continuous Performance Test assessed by positron emission tomography.Br J Psychiatry. 1990;156216-227
37.Jonides
JSmith
ESKoeppe
RAAwh
EMinoshima
SMintun
MASpatial working memory in humans as revealed by PET.ٳܰ. 1993;363583-584
38.Berman
KFOstrem
JLRandolph
CGolde
JGoldberg
TECoppola
RCarson
REHerscovitch
PWeinberger
DRPhysiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: a positron emission tomography study.ܰDZ⳦DZDz. 1995;331027-1046
39.Ridley
RMThe psychology of perserverative and stereotyped behaviour.Prog Neurobiol. 1994;44221-231
40.Crawford
TJPuri
BKNijran
KSJones
BKennard
CLewis
SWAbnormal saccadic distractibility in patients with schizophrenia: a 99m Tc-HMPAO SPET study.Psychol Med. 1996;26265-277
41.O'Driscoll
GABenkelfat
CJoober
RLal
SMatthysse
SWHolzman
PSFlorencio
PSEvans
ACRegional brain activity correlated with pursuit gain in the first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients.
Soc Neurosci Abstr. 1996;Abstract 468.15
Google Scholar 42.Steffy
RAWaldman
ISchizophrenics' reaction time: North star or shooting star?Cromwell
RSnyder
Ceds.Schizophrenia Origins, Processes, Treatment, and Outcome New York, NY Oxford University Press1993;111-134
43.Fukushima
JMorita
NFukushima
KChiba
TTanaka
SYamashita
IVoluntary control of saccadic eye movements in patients with schizophrenic and affective disorders.J Psychiatr Res. 1990;249-24
44.Fukushima
JFukushima
KMorita
NYamashita
IDisturbances in the control of saccadic eye movement and eye-head coordination in schizophrenics.J Vestib Res. 1990-1991;1171-80
45.Lencz
TRaine
AScerbo
ARedmon
MBrodish
SHolt
LBird
LImpaired eye tracking in undergraduates with schizotypal personality disorder.Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150152-154
46.Siever
LJFriedman
LMoskowitz
JMitropoulou
VKeefe
RSRoitman
SLMerhige
DTrestman
RSilverman
JMohs
RCEye movement impairment and schizotypal psychopathology.Am J Psychiatry. 1994;1511209-1215
47.Kendler
KSOchs
ALGorman
AMHewitt
JKRoss
DEMirsky
AFThe structure of schizotypy: a pilot multitrait twin study.Psychiatry Res. 1991;3619-36
48.Katsanis
JIacono
WClinical, neuropsychological and brain structural correlates of smooth-pursuit eye tracking performance in chronic schizophrenics.J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100526-534
49.Ross
DEThaker
GKBuchanan
RWLahti
ACMedoff
DBartko
JJMoran
MHartley
JAssociation of abnormal smooth pursuit eye movements with the deficit syndrome in schizophrenic patients.Am J Psychiatry. 1996;1531158-1165
50.Ciuffreda
KJAlpert
MBlackstone
TFudge
RThaler
JPursuit eye movements in chronic schizophrenics: relationship between increased saccades and negative symptoms.Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1994;1479-81
51.van den Bosch
RJEye tracking impairment: attentional and psychometric correlates in psychiatric patients.J Psychiatr Res. 1984;18277-286
52.Keefe
RSSiever
LJMohs
RCEye tracking, schizophrenic symptoms, and schizotypal personality disorder.Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci. 1989;23939-42
53.Iacono
WGLykken
DTEye tracking and psychopathology: new procedures applied to a sample of normal monozygotic twins.Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1979;361361-1369
54.Coursey
RDLees
RWSiever
LJThe relationship between smooth pursuit eye movement impairment and psychological measures of psychopathology.Psychol Med. 1989;19343-358
55.van den Bosch
RJRozendaal
NMol
JMSymptom correlates of eye tracking dysfunction.Biol Psychiatry. 1987;22919-921
56.Simons
RFKatkin
WSmooth pursuit eye movements in subjects reporting physical anhedonia and perceptual aberrations.Psychiatry Res. 1985;14275-289
57.Lenzenweger
MFSchizotypy and schizotypic psychopathology: mapping an alternative expression of schizophrenia liability.Lenzenweger
MDworkin
Reds.Origins and Development of Schizophrenia Advances in Experimental Psychopathology Washington, DC American Psychological Association1998;93-121
58.Holzman
PSThe role of psychological probes in genetic studies of schizophrenia.Schizophr Res. 1994;131-9
59.Franke
PMaier
WHardt
JHain
CCornblatt
BAAttentional abilities and measures of schizotypy: their variation and covariation in schizophrenic patients, their siblings, and normal control subjects.Psychiatry Res. 1994;54259-272
60.Thaker
GMoran
MAdami
HCassady
SPsychosis proneness scales in schizophrenia spectrum personality disorders: familial vs. non-familial samples.Psychiatry Res. 1993;4647-57
61.Clementz
BAGrove
WMKatsanis
JIacono
WGPsychometric detection of schizotypy: perceptual aberration and physical anhedonia in relatives of schizophrenics.J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100607-612