Key PointsQuestionÌý
What is the extent and impact of lead-contaminated drinking water in Chicago, Illinois?
FindingsÌý
In this cross-sectional study, an estimated 68% of children younger than 6 years in Chicago are exposed to lead-contaminated drinking water, with 19% of affected children using unfiltered tap water as their primary drinking water source. Predominantly Black and Hispanic blocks were disproportionately less likely to be tested for lead yet disproportionately exposed to contaminated drinking water.
MeaningÌý
Childhood lead exposure from drinking water is widespread in Chicago, with racial inequities in both testing rates and exposure levels.
ImportanceÌý
There is no level of lead in drinking water considered to be safe, yet lead service lines are still commonly used in water systems across the US.
ObjectiveÌý
To identify the extent of lead-contaminated drinking water in Chicago, Illinois, and model its impact on children younger than 6 years.
Design, Setting, and ParticipantsÌý
For this cross-sectional study, a retrospective assessment was performed of lead exposure based on household tests collected from January 2016 to September 2023. Tests were obtained from households in Chicago that registered for a free self-administered testing service for lead exposure. Machine learning and microsimulation were used to estimate citywide childhood lead exposure.
ExposureÌý
Lead-contaminated drinking water, measured in parts per billion.
Main Outcomes and MeasuresÌý
Number of children younger than 6 years exposed to lead-contaminated water.
ResultsÌý
A total of 38 385 household lead tests were collected. An estimated 68% (95% uncertainty interval, 66%-69%) of children younger than 6 years were exposed to lead-contaminated water, corresponding to 129 000 children (95% uncertainty interval, 128 000-131 000 children). Ten-percentage-point increases in block-level Black and Hispanic populations were associated with 3% (95% CI, 2%-3%) and 6% (95% CI, 5%-7%) decreases in odds of being tested for lead and 4% (95% CI, 3%-6%) and 11% (95% CI, 10%-13%) increases in having lead-contaminated drinking water, respectively.
Conclusions and RelevanceÌý
These findings indicate that childhood lead exposure is widespread in Chicago, and racial inequities are present in both testing rates and exposure levels. Machine learning may assist in preliminary screening for lead exposure, and efforts to remediate the effects of environmental racism should involve improving outreach for and access to lead testing services.
Lead exposure has been known to pose significant health risks, leading to historically successful large-scale efforts to reduce lead exposure from common sources, such as gasoline or paint, in the US. However, lead contamination in drinking water remains a public health concern that has garnered heightened attention in recent years.1 Lead pipes, which were commonly used in water systems until their federal ban in 1986, can leach lead into the water supply, especially when the water contains corrosive materials.1,2 There is no level of lead in drinking water considered to be safe for consumption, with the Environmental Protection Agency setting the maximum contaminant level goal for lead in water at 0.3 Even low-level exposure to lead-contaminated drinking water has been found to be associated with increased blood lead levels (BLLs), and even modestly elevated BLLs are associated with adverse outcomes.4-11 Lead exposure can have serious health consequences, particularly for children, including developmental deficits, cardiovascular complications, chronic kidney disease, and neurologic complications.1,2,5,12,13
Many cities across the US still have lead service lines in their water systems, including Chicago, Illinois, where lead pipes were mandated until the 1986 federal ban.14 Chicago is estimated to have nearly 400 000 lead service lines (where 1 line serves approximately 1 household), the most of any US city.15 Despite efforts to identify and replace lead service lines, progress has been slow, with only 280 (0.007%) lead pipes replaced by the city government from 2020 to 2022. To collect data on lead-contaminated water, the city offers free self-testing kits for residents. However, such a data collection approach may be subject to selection bias, potentially obscuring the true prevalence of lead exposure.
This cross-sectional retrospective study examined the extent and impact of childhood lead exposure across Chicago on the basis of household tests. Machine learning, regression, and microsimulation models were used to estimate the prevalence of childhood lead exposure from drinking water, identify racial inequities in terms of lead prevalence and screening, and model potential BLL increases in children exposed to lead-contaminated drinking water.
This cross-sectional study did not require review based on the Johns Hopkins Institutional Review Board process, as it does not meet the criteria for human participant research. Specifically, this study was a secondary data analysis of deidentified, delinked data, and investigators had no role in its original collection. Informed consent does not apply because this was not human participant research. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology () guideline was followed in the design of this study. The eMethods in Supplement 1 provide more information on all aspects of the study setting, models, and analyses.
We used publicly available lead testing data provided by the Chicago Department of Water Management containing 38 385 lead tests collected from January 2016 to September 2023. Tests were partially anonymized with the last 2 digits of each address truncated; the tests encompassed 12 139 census blocks and at least 14 673 unique addresses.16 Each test contained at least 3 distinct measurements of lead concentrations: a first draw after water had been kept stagnant for at least 6 hours, a second draw after 2 minutes of flushing, and a third draw after 5 minutes of flushing.
We obtained sociodemographic information using 5-year estimates from the 2021 American Community Survey17 and the 2020 Census,18 including information such as population, race, education, poverty, building age, and home value. Race and ethnicity were categorized as Asian, Black, Hispanic, and White. We also used data from the Chicago Health Atlas19 for tract-level health metrics, and data on building footprints were obtained from the City of Chicago.20 We obtained citywide survey responses to the Healthy Chicago Survey in aggregate for years 2021 to 2022 from the Chicago Department of Public Health, where respondents were asked whether their primary source of drinking water was from unfiltered tap water, filtered tap water, bottled water, or some other source, aggregated by neighborhood. Our data set consisted of 33 786 residential census blocks in total, selected by filtering for blocks with nonzero block populations within Chicago. We assessed the characteristics of census blocks in our study by aggregating over block-level information, such as population by race and ethnic group and building features. An overview of our data sources and analytical strategy is in eFigure 1 and eTable 1 in Supplement 1.
Estimating Risk of Lead Exposure
We trained machine learning models to determine the block-level risk of having lead-contaminated drinking water, defined as the majority (≥50%) of tests within a block having 1 part per billion (ppb) or more of lead concentration in the second draw. The value of 1 ppb was chosen as the decision threshold because (1) no amount of lead in drinking water is considered safe for consumption, and (2) 1 ppb is the limit of detection for the lead water tests, making it a natural threshold for determining whether a household has lead-contaminated drinking water. We chose the second draw as an outcome because it had the highest median lead concentration, suiting our intent to identify households with lead-contaminated drinking water.
We tuned our models via cross-validation on a training set using LightGBM,21 a gradient-boosting decision tree algorithm, as our final model and used block-level sociodemographic and building age variables as predictors (eTable 1 in Supplement 1). Model performance was evaluated on the basis of predictions on a held-out data set that was never used for model training. As robustness checks, we (1) trained models using tests as the unit of observation instead of census blocks and (2) trained models using the first and third draws instead of the second draw. We computed model explanations using Shapley Additive Explanations, a method to interpret the output of machine learning models by decomposing predictions into additive feature contributions.22,23
Assessing Disparities in Lead Screening and Exposure
To assess racial disparities in lead screening, we conducted risk-adjusted logistic regressions,24 a method leveraging machine learning output to measure disparities while adjusting for omitted-variable and included-variable bias. The outcome was whether a census block was screened, and covariates included the proportion of block-level population by racial and ethnic group and predictions of estimated risk of lead exposure from our machine learning model. We included estimated risk as a covariate to explicitly assess whether testing disparities exist even after adjusting for risk of lead exposure. As a robustness check, we conducted regressions without adjusting for estimated risk.
To assess disparities in lead exposure, we conducted logistic regressions with lead exposure as a binary outcome and with block-level population by racial and ethnic group and block-level population overall as covariates. Regressions were conducted separately for each racial and ethnic group. For all regressions, we calculated E-values, which indicate the minimum strength an unobserved confounder would need to have to explain away the association identified from the regression using the calculation method for odds ratios with common outcomes.25 As a robustness check, we conducted exposure odds analyses using tests as the unit of observation instead of census blocks.
Estimating Extent of Childhood Exposure to Lead-Contaminated Drinking Water
To estimate the extent of childhood exposure to lead-contaminated drinking water per block, we used a Monte Carlo microsimulation approach with 10 000 simulations. To model block-level child population younger than 6 years, we used the children younger than 5 years and children younger than 10 years variables from the American Community Survey; we calculated the number of 5-year-old children as the number of 5- to 9-year-old children divided by 5, assuming children were equally distributed across years of age. We classified each block as having either lead exposure or no lead exposure based on output from our machine learning models and adjusted for misclassifications by sampling from a binomial distribution based on the known positive and negative predictive values of our machine learning model. We then determined number of children exposed by summing the number of children over all blocks modeled as having lead exposure; we multiplied these estimates by block-level measurements of race and ethnicity and reported primary source of drinking water to obtain stratified estimates. For blocks modeled as having nonzero lead exposure, we modeled drinking water lead concentration by aggregating all test results with nonzero lead concentration from their respective geographic regions and sampled from them with replacement.
We calculated the relative increase of BLL attributable to lead-contaminated drinking water for blocks modeled as having lead exposure by using an identified exposure-response association between lead-contaminated drinking water and BLL in children aged 1 to 5 years in Montreal, Quebec, Canada, after 150 days of exposure.11 To do this, we sampled from a uniform distribution centered on their estimate for the increase in BLL per ppb of lead in drinking water, bounded by the 95% CIs of the estimate. Because their estimates were based on lead-contaminated water samples that did not exceed 10 ppb, we conservatively top-coded all concentration observations above 10 ppb to be 10 ppb (10% of observations were above 10 ppb). We chose their most conservative specification for the effect of lead in water on BLL, using adjustments for other forms of lead exposure. As robustness checks, we ran our microsimulations using the unadjusted exposure-response association between lead water concentration and BLL using probabilistic estimations and using household lead tests as the observation unit instead of census blocks. All parenthetical measures of uncertainty for results from our simulations are reported in 95% uncertainty intervals, or the interval between the 5th and 95th percentiles of values over all simulations.
All the analyses were performed with the use of R software, version 3.6.3 (R Project for Statistical Computing). Additional details are provided in Supplement 1.
Population Characteristics
Our lead testing data set included 38 385 household lead tests. Census blocks with testing results encompassed 36% of 33 786 residential census blocks. Tested and untested blocks had different racial, ethnic, and geographic distributions (Table 1 and Figure) but similar median building ages. Sixty-nine percent of lead tests yielded 1 ppb or greater lead concentration, and 33% of lead tests yielded 5 ppb or greater lead concentration (Table 2). Among 8360 citywide survey respondents, 1859 (weighted percentage, 20%) used unfiltered tap water as their primary source of drinking water (eFigure 2 and eTable 2 in Supplement 1).
Lead Exposure Risk Estimates
The machine learning model was able to predict lead exposure on a held-out test set with an area under the receiver operating characteristic curve of 0.81 (eTable 3 in Supplement 1). A total of 75% of all census blocks were estimated to have lead-contaminated drinking water (eTable 4 in Supplement 1). The top predictors were geographic units (community area, census tract, and census block group), block population, and number of buildings per block (eFigure 3 in Supplement 1). Blocks with larger populations had lower estimated risk, and blocks with a higher number of buildings per block had higher estimated risk.
Disparities in Screening Rates and Lead Exposure
Racial disparities in screening rates and lead exposure were present. Ten-percentage-point increases in block-level Black and Hispanic populations were associated with 3% (95% CI, 2%-3%) and 6% (95% CI, 5%-7%) decreases in screening odds, respectively; a 10% increase in White population was associated with a 24% (95% CI, 23%-25%) increase in screening odds. Concurrently, 10-percentage-point increases in Black and Hispanic populations were associated with 4% (95% CI, 3%-6%) and 11% (95% CI, 10%-13%) increases in lead exposure, respectively; a 10-percentage-point increase in block-level White population was associated with a 5% (95% CI, 4%-6%) decrease (Table 3; eFigure 4 and eTable 5 in Supplement 1). Black and Hispanic survey respondents reported the lowest rates of unfiltered tap water use (weighted percentages, 14% and 12%, respectively), and White respondents reported the highest (weighted percentage, 32%) (eTable 2 in Supplement 1).
Childhood Lead Exposure Estimates
Our machine learning model estimated 75% of 33 786 residential census blocks to have lead-contaminated water (Figure). A total of 129 000 of all children younger than 6 years (68%; 95% uncertainty interval, 66%-69%) were estimated to be exposed to lead-contaminated water. An estimated 19% of exposed children (n = 22 400) used unfiltered tap water as their primary drinking water source, corresponding to an estimated 103% (95% uncertainty interval, 46.7%-162%) increase in BLLs after 150 days of exposure (Table 4 and eTables 6-8 in Supplement 1).
In this cross-sectional retrospective study, childhood exposure to lead-contaminated drinking water was estimated to be widespread in Chicago. We estimated that more than two-thirds of children are exposed to lead-contaminated drinking water, and among those exposed, 19% use unfiltered tap water as their main source of drinking water. We observed from our models that long-term consumption of contaminated water could lead to substantial increases in BLLs.
Increased BLLs in children can cause deficits in cognitive development and other adverse health outcomes.4-7,12,13,26 The impact of low-level, long-term exposure to lead-contaminated drinking water may not be easily identifiable at the individual level. Instead, it could cause population-level increases in adverse health outcomes, such as lower population-level mean IQ or increased preterm births, underscoring the need for reduced exposure to lead-contaminated drinking water.5,12,13,26
Our findings on primary drinking water sources by race and ethnic group corroborate prior findings that Black and Hispanic households disproportionately drink bottled water, and White households disproportionately drink tap water.27 However, bottled water is not necessarily less lead contaminated than tap water; the US Food and Drug Administration sets the lead concentration limit in bottled water to 5 ppb. Similarly, using filtered tap water does not necessarily prevent lead exposure, as many consumer-grade filters do not remove lead, and some households may not change their filters as consistently as is required. The extent to which groups primarily using filtered tap water or bottled water are exposed to lead-contaminated drinking water is therefore unknown.
The racial and ethnic disparities present are indicative of the myriad ways environmental racism can manifest. Lower screening rates, lower consumption of tap water, and higher levels of lead exposure among predominantly Black and Hispanic blocks may indicate mistrust toward water sources or lack of community engagement from relevant authorities.27-30 Neighborhoods with high-risk estimates as well as low screening rates were largely clustered in the south and west sides of the city, corresponding to the city’s geographic history of segregation and disinvestment (Figure).7,31-33
This study contributes to the existing literature by estimating population-level exposure to and relative BLL increase attributable to lead-contaminated drinking water. Prior studies have simulated impacts of all-source lead exposure and identified associations between lead-contaminated drinking water and BLLs, the results of which enabled our modeling approach.6,11-13 Other studies have used machine learning to detect lead service lines34 or determine lead poisoning rates35,36; one of these studies, conducted in Chicago, achieved a validated area under the receiver operating characteristic curve of 0.69 in estimating elevated BLLs in children.35 Our study extends the literature by using machine learning models to estimate out-of-sample population-level prevalence of lead exposure and model population-level impacts of exposure.
Some limitations to our study include data resolution and missingness. Lead contamination levels were partially anonymized by truncating the last 2 digits of each address, so we were unable to incorporate household-specific data; an analysis done on the deanonymized data set would likely have stronger predictive performance. Second, we used machine learning to estimate the risk of lead contamination for households that did not test for lead, but this approach assumes the relationship between variables and lead exposure to be the same between the observable data and the out-of-sample data; actual data from those households may vary, and equitable screening and data collection for all at-risk neighborhoods should remain a priority to reduce lead exposure. Third, we do not have representative data on BLLs across the city, meaning we are only able to estimate relative increases in BLL. This lack of data in combination with lack of sufficiently identified dose-response associations led us to not model many health outcomes from lead exposure, such as IQ loss, maternal health, cardiovascular outcomes, or developmental disorders.4-7,12,13,26 For example, low-level lead exposure has been shown to be associated with increased preterm births,26 but we are unable to model these outcomes due to lack of (1) representative information on baseline BLLs and (2) a suitable exposure-response function for lead-contaminated drinking water and BLLs among pregnant women.
Other limitations involve modeling assumptions. Our model estimates of relative BLL increase are based on a Montreal study on children aged 1 to 5 years and assume generalizability of the exposure-response function to children in Chicago. The geometric mean of BLL in the Montreal study was 13.4 μg/L (95% CI, 5.0-36.1 μg/L). The mean BLL in Chicago is not known, but the reported Illinois geometric mean in 2019 was 21 μg/L,37 and the reported national geometric mean was 8.3 μg/L (95% CI, 7.8-8.8 μg/L) from 2011 to 2016,38 neither of which corresponds exactly to the Montreal mean but do fall within its CIs. The mean age of children from the Montreal study is 41 months; our modeled child population assumes an even distribution across years of age, with a mean age of 42 months. To account for these potential population differences, we presented our estimates of BLL increases across a wide interval of possible values to represent model uncertainty. Our model also assumes children in Chicago have similar water consumption habits and age distributions as children from the Montreal study. Our estimates of relative BLL increase only apply to children aged 1 to 5 years, as infants up to 12 months old were excluded from the Montreal study and consume more water relative to their body weight. Similarly, our estimates of BLL increases cannot be extrapolated beyond 150 days, as the original study only calculated lead exposure to 150 days preceding BLL measurements.39
A last category of study limitations is that our study does not investigate sources of lead exposure beyond drinking water. Racial and ethnic disparities in elevated BLLs in children may be driven by other environmental factors, such as dust and paint for Black and Hispanic children and imported food for Asian children.33,40-44 Efforts to reduce childhood lead exposure should take a holistic view across different environmental factors and sociocultural contexts.
Levels of widespread childhood lead exposure, such as those found in this study, are symptomatic of structural marginalization and are likely preventable through large-scale interventions to replace lead service lines and improve access to testing. The benefits of harm-reduction strategies, such as lead filtration technology and anticorrosive agents to prevent lead leaching into water, should also be studied and explored. Machine learning may be useful as a preliminary screening tool, and a holistic approach to supplement data-driven identification with community-based input could help prevent lead exposure. Further action should be taken to reduce childhood lead exposure from drinking water.
Accepted for Publication: January 12, 2024.
Published Online: March 18, 2024. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2024.0133
Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2024 Huynh BQ et al. JAMA Pediatrics.
Corresponding Author: Benjamin Q. Huynh, PhD, Department of Environmental Health and Engineering, Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health, 615 N Wolfe St, Room E7030, Baltimore, MD 21205 (bhuynh@jhu.edu).
Author Contributions: Prof Huynh had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Concept and design: Huynh.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.
Drafting of the manuscript: Huynh.
Critical review of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.
Statistical analysis: Huynh, Chin.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Huynh, Kiang.
Supervision: Kiang.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.
Additional Contributions: We thank Emile Jorgensen, MPH (Chicago Department of Public Health), for helpful discussions. We thank the Chicago Department of Public Health for providing access to the survey data on tap water use. No compensation was provided.
Data Sharing Statement: See Supplement 2.
1.Hanna-Attisha
ÌýM, LaChance
ÌýJ, Sadler
ÌýRC, Champney Schnepp
ÌýA. ÌýElevated blood lead levels in children associated with the Flint drinking water crisis: a spatial analysis of risk and public health response.Ìý ÌýAm J Public Health. 2016;106(2):283-290. doi:
2.Burki
ÌýT. ÌýLead’s lingering legacy.Ìý ÌýLancet Child Adolesc Health. 2023;7(6):375-376. doi:
3.United States Environmental Protection Agency. Lead and copper rule. October 13, 2015. Accessed December 26, 2023.
4.Canfield
ÌýRL, Henderson
ÌýCR
ÌýJr, Cory-Slechta
ÌýDA, Cox
ÌýC, Jusko
ÌýTA, Lanphear
ÌýBP. ÌýIntellectual impairment in children with blood lead concentrations below 10 microg per deciliter.Ìý ÌýN Engl J Med. 2003;348(16):1517-1526. doi:
5.Reuben
ÌýA, Caspi
ÌýA, Belsky
ÌýDW,
Ìýet al. ÌýAssociation of childhood blood lead levels with cognitive function and socioeconomic status at age 38 years and with IQ change and socioeconomic mobility between childhood and adulthood.Ìý Ìý´³´¡²Ñ´¡. 2017;317(12):1244-1251. doi:
6.Lanphear
ÌýBP, Hornung
ÌýR, Khoury
ÌýJ,
Ìýet al. ÌýLow-level environmental lead exposure and children’s intellectual function: an international pooled analysis.Ìý ÌýEnviron Health Perspect. 2005;113(7):894-899. doi:
7.Evens
ÌýA, Hryhorczuk
ÌýD, Lanphear
ÌýBP,
Ìýet al. ÌýThe impact of low-level lead toxicity on school performance among children in the Chicago Public Schools: a population-based retrospective cohort study.Ìý ÌýEnviron Health. 2015;14(1):21. doi:
8.Levallois
ÌýP, St-Laurent
ÌýJ, Gauvin
ÌýD,
Ìýet al. ÌýThe impact of drinking water, indoor dust and paint on blood lead levels of children aged 1-5 years in Montréal (Québec, Canada).Ìý ÌýJ Expo Sci Environ Epidemiol. 2014;24(2):185-191. doi:
9.Lanphear
ÌýBP, Burgoon
ÌýDA, Rust
ÌýSW, Eberly
ÌýS, Galke
ÌýW. ÌýEnvironmental exposures to lead and urban children’s blood lead levels.Ìý ÌýEnviron Res. 1998;76(2):120-130. doi:
10.Lanphear
ÌýBP, Hornung
ÌýR, Ho
ÌýM, Howard
ÌýCR, Eberly
ÌýS, Knauf
ÌýK. ÌýEnvironmental lead exposure during early childhood.Ìý ÌýJ Pediatr. 2002;140(1):40-47. doi:
11.Ngueta
ÌýG, Abdous
ÌýB, Tardif
ÌýR, St-Laurent
ÌýJ, Levallois
ÌýP. ÌýUse of a cumulative exposure index to estimate the impact of tap water lead concentration on blood lead levels in 1- to 5-year-old children (Montréal, Canada).Ìý ÌýEnviron Health Perspect. 2016;124(3):388-395. doi:
12.Gould
ÌýE. ÌýChildhood lead poisoning: conservative estimates of the social and economic benefits of lead hazard control.Ìý ÌýEnviron Health Perspect. 2009;117(7):1162-1167. doi:
13.McFarland
ÌýMJ, Hauer
ÌýME, Reuben
ÌýA. ÌýHalf of US population exposed to adverse lead levels in early childhood.Ìý ÌýProc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2022;119(11):e2118631119. doi:
14.Lead-pipe issue surfaces again. Chicago Tribune. March 30, 1986. Accessed September 22, 2023.
15.Despite a promise, Chicago has made no progress on removal of lead pipes. WBEZ Chicago. April 18, 2021. Accessed September 22, 2023.
16.Chicago Department of Water Management. Chicago water quality. 2023. Accessed November 15, 2023.
17.US Census Bureau. American Community Survey. 2023. Accessed November 15, 2023.
18.US Census Bureau. 2020 Census. 2021. Accessed November 15, 2023.
19.Chicago Department of Public Health, UIC Phame Center, Metopio. Chicago Health Atlas. 2023. Accessed November 15, 2023.
20.City of Chicago Data Portal. Chicago building footprints. 2023. Accessed November 15, 2023.
21.Ke
ÌýG, Meng
ÌýQ, Finley
ÌýT,
Ìýet al. LightGBM: a highly efficient gradient boosting decision tree. In: Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems. Curran Associates Inc; 2017;30. Accessed July 13, 2023.
22.Lundberg
ÌýSM, Lee
ÌýSI. A unified approach to interpreting model predictions. In: Proceedings of the 31st International Conference on Neural Information Processing Systems. NIPS’17. Curran Associates Inc; 2017:4768-4777.
23.Lundberg
ÌýSM, Erion
ÌýG, Chen
ÌýH,
Ìýet al. ÌýFrom local explanations to global understanding with explainable AI for trees.Ìý ÌýNat Mach Intell. 2020;2(1):56-67. doi:
24.Jung J, Corbett-Davies S, Shroff R, Goel S. ÌýOmitted and included variable bias in tests for disparate impact.Ìý arXiv. Preprint posted online August 29, 2019. doi:
25.VanderWeele
ÌýTJ, Ding
ÌýP. ÌýSensitivity analysis in observational research: introducing the E-value.Ìý ÌýAnn Intern Med. 2017;167(4):268-274. doi:
26.Fisher
ÌýM, Marro
ÌýL, Arbuckle
ÌýTE,
Ìýet al. ÌýAssociation between toxic metals, vitamin D and preterm birth in the maternal-infant research on environmental chemicals study.Ìý ÌýPaediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2023;37(5):447-457. doi:
27.Gorelick
ÌýMH, Gould
ÌýL, Nimmer
ÌýM,
Ìýet al. ÌýPerceptions about water and increased use of bottled water in minority children.Ìý ÌýArch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2011;165(10):928-932. doi:
28.Ezell
ÌýJM, Chase
ÌýEC. ÌýForming a critical race theory of environmental disaster: understanding social meanings and health threat perception in the Flint water crisis.Ìý ÌýJ Environ Manage. 2022;320:115886. doi:
29.Weisner
ÌýML, Root
ÌýTL, Harris
ÌýMS, Mitsova
ÌýD, Liu
ÌýW. ÌýTap water perceptions and socioeconomics: assessing the dissatisfaction of the poor.Ìý ÌýSustain Prod Consum. 2020;21:269-278. doi:
30.Williams
ÌýBL, Florez
ÌýY. ÌýDo Mexican Americans perceive environmental issues differently than Caucasians: a study of cross-ethnic variation in perceptions related to water in Tucson.Ìý ÌýEnviron Health Perspect. 2002;110(suppl 2):303-310. doi:
31.Kramer
ÌýMR, Hogue
ÌýCR. ÌýIs segregation bad for your health?Ìý ÌýEpidemiol Rev. 2009;31(1):178-194. doi:
32.Orsi
ÌýJM, Margellos-Anast
ÌýH, Whitman
ÌýS. ÌýBlack-White health disparities in the United States and Chicago: a 15-year progress analysis.Ìý ÌýAm J Public Health. 2010;100(2):349-356. doi:
33.Sampson
ÌýRJ, Winter
ÌýAS. ÌýTHE racial ecology of lead poisoning: toxic inequality in Chicago neighborhoods, 1995-2013.Ìý ÌýBois Rev Soc Sci Res Race. 2016;13(2):261-283. doi:
34.Abernethy
ÌýJ, Chojnacki
ÌýA, Farahi
ÌýA, Schwartz
ÌýE, Webb
ÌýJ. ActiveRemediation: the search for lead pipes in Flint, Michigan. In: Proceedings of the 24th ACM SIGKDD International Conference on Knowledge Discovery & Data Mining (KDD ‘18). Association for Computing Machinery. 2018:5-14.
35.Potash
ÌýE, Ghani
ÌýR, Walsh
ÌýJ,
Ìýet al. ÌýValidation of a machine learning model to predict childhood lead poisoning.Ìý Ìý´³´¡²Ñ´¡ Netw Open. 2020;3(9):e2012734. doi:
36.Lobo
ÌýGP, Kalyan
ÌýB, Gadgil
ÌýAJ. ÌýPredicting childhood lead exposure at an aggregated level using machine learning.Ìý ÌýInt J Hyg Environ Health. 2021;238:113862. doi:
37.Fokum
ÌýFD, Estrop T, McAfee K, Butler GC. Illinois Lead Program 2019 Annual Surveillance Report. Illinois Department of Public Health. 2020. Accessed November 15, 2023.
38.Ruckart
ÌýPZ, Jones
ÌýRL, Courtney
ÌýJG,
Ìýet al. ÌýUpdate of the blood lead reference value—United States, 2021.Ìý ÌýMMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2021;70(43):1509-1512. doi:
39.Ershow
ÌýAG. Total Water and Tapwater Intake in the United States: Population-Based Estimates of Quantities and Sources. Life Sciences Research Office, Federation of American Societies for Experimental Biology; 1989.
40.Lanphear
ÌýBP, Weitzman
ÌýM, Eberly
ÌýS. ÌýRacial differences in urban children’s environmental exposures to lead.Ìý ÌýAm J Public Health. 1996;86(10):1460-1463. doi:
41.Teye
ÌýSO, Yanosky
ÌýJD, Cuffee
ÌýY,
Ìýet al. ÌýExploring persistent racial/ethnic disparities in lead exposure among American children aged 1-5 years: results from NHANES 1999-2016.Ìý ÌýInt Arch Occup Environ Health. 2021;94(4):723-730. doi:
42.White
ÌýBM, Bonilha
ÌýHS, Ellis
ÌýC
ÌýJr. ÌýRacial/ethnic differences in childhood blood lead levels among children <72 months of age in the United States: a systematic review of the literature.Ìý ÌýJ Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2016;3(1):145-153. doi:
43.Cowell
ÌýW, Ireland
ÌýT, Vorhees
ÌýD, Heiger-Bernays
ÌýW. ÌýGround turmeric as a source of lead exposure in the United States.Ìý ÌýPublic Health Rep. 2017;132(3):289-293. doi:
44.Hore
ÌýP, Ahmed
ÌýMS, Sedlar
ÌýS, Saper
ÌýRB, Nagin
ÌýD, Clark
ÌýN. ÌýBlood lead levels and potential risk factors for lead exposures among South Asians in New York City.Ìý ÌýJ Immigr Minor Health. 2017;19(6):1322-1329. doi: